共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
F. Lynn Carpenter Mark A. Hixon Robert W. Russell David C. Paton Ethan J. Temeles 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1993,33(5):297-304
Summary Three age-sex classes of rufous hummingbirds (Selasphorus rufus) overlap temporally and defend feeding territories during migratory stopovers in the Sierra Nevada of California. We demonstrate that these classes differ in their ability to secure and maintain high-quality feeding territories for refueling, and that these differences result in differences in resource use. Data on acquisition of territories, territory characteristics, and responses of territory owners to intruders suggest that several mechanisms are involved in determining dominance, involving sex- and age-related differences in wing disc loading, coloration, and experience. We discuss the implications of these results for understanding intraspecific variation in migration strategies.
Correspondence to: F.L. Carpenter 相似文献
2.
Behavioral and demographic changes following the loss of the breeding female in cooperatively breeding marmosets 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
C. Lazaro-Perea C. S. S. Castro R. Harrison A. Araujo M. F. Arruda C. T. Snowdon 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2000,48(2):137-146
Recent models of the evolution and dynamics of family structure in cooperatively breeding vertebrates predict that the opening
of breeding vacancies in cooperatively breeding groups will result in (1) dispersal movements to fill the reproductive position,
and (2) within-group conflict over access to reproduction. We describe the behavioral and demographic changes that followed
the creation of breeding vacancies in three wild groups of cooperatively breeding common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus). Replacement of the breeding female was rapid when no adult females remained in the group, but did not occur for several
months when other adult females were present. Aggression of adult animals towards same-sex potential immigrants was associated
with a period of reduced affiliation, increased intragroup agonism, no intragroup sexual behavior, and frequent extragroup
copulations. This ended with the fissioning of groups along sexual lines. After replacement, multiple males copulated with
multiple females and vice versa, with no increases in sexually related aggression. Female-female conflict was resolved through
infanticide. The lack of direct conflict between males is consistent with cooperative polyandry. After a breeding vacancy
appeared, marmoset groups showed conflict of interests among group members similar to those shown by cooperatively breeding
birds, but they used different behavioral mechanisms to resolve those conflicts. Our data provide important evidence from
a cooperatively breeding mammal to support Emlen’s model for the evolution of vertebrate families, but they suggest that species-specific
inter- and intrasexual competitive strategies should be considered before the model can be applied to other cooperatively
breeding vertebrates.
Received: 23 November 1999 / Received in revised form: 28 March 2000 / Accepted: 1 April 2000 相似文献
3.
Size assessment in simulated territorial encounters between male green frogs (Rana clamitans) 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Mark A. Bee Stephen A. Perrill Patrick C. Owen 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1999,45(3-4):177-184
We examined the ability of male green frogs to assess the size of an opponent based on the dominant frequency of their advertisement
call, which is negatively correlated with size, using synthetic stimuli to simulate intruders of different sizes. In one field
playback experiment, we broadcast a pair of stimuli representing a small and a large male; in a second experiment, we broadcast
calls of a medium and a large male. In both experiments, males produced calls with significantly lower dominant frequencies
in response to each stimulus. Contrast analyses revealed that males lowered the dominant frequency of their calls more in
response to the large-male stimulus than in responses to the small- and medium-male stimuli. In the second experiment, males
also responded to the large-male stimulus by calling at higher rates. There were no differences in mean note duration or the
number of moves made toward or around the playback speaker in response to any stimulus. Thus, the frequency of an opponent's
calls elicits a differential modification of calling behavior, primarily in the form of differential dominant frequency alteration,
suggesting that males use dominant frequency to assess the size of opponents during aggressive encounters.
Received: 17 April 1998 / Accepted after revision: 7 October 1998 相似文献
4.
In mammalian polygynous mating systems, male reproductive effort consists mainly of male–male competition and courting of
females, which entail substantial somatic costs. Males are thus expected to adjust their reproductive effort according to
their age and condition. In this study, we examined how activity budgets of male mountain goats (Oreamnos americanus), a polygynous ungulate, varied with age in a marked population over two periods: (1) summers 1995–2006 and (2) ruts 2004–2006.
We then assessed if the proportions of time spent in male–male competition and courtship behaviors were influenced by age-specific
body mass and social rank during the rut. Males spent most of their time foraging and resting during summer, and rested more
and foraged less with increasing age. During the rut, pronounced shifts in activity budgets occurred as juveniles (1–2 years)
increased time spent foraging, whereas adults (≥3 years) increased standing and time spent in social interactions at the expense
of foraging. At old age, reproductive effort either stabilized or decreased slightly, providing weak support for the ‘mating
strategy–effort’ hypothesis, predicting that courtship behaviors should peak in prime-aged males. Age-specific body mass did
not affect time spent in male–male competition, but was positively related with time spent in courtship behaviors, providing
support for the ‘individual quality’ hypothesis, predicting that males with more resources at the start of the rut should
spend more time in mating-related activities. Age-specific social rank did not affect reproductive effort. Surviving to prime
age while increasing mass each year should thus allow male ungulates to gain greater ability to court estrus females. 相似文献
5.
Dominance status influences the fitness of many mammals. Using African striped mice Rhabdomys pumilio, we tested whether (1) dominant females have greater reproductive success than subordinate females, (2) dominant females
influence the reproductive output of subordinate females when they are housed in close proximity, (3) reproductive output
of a female changes in response to the dominance status of her neighbours, and (4) whether prolonged association between individuals
influences the variance in reproductive success between dominants and subordinates (i.e. the ‘dear enemy’ phenomenon). The
size and mass of litters of dominants increased significantly when housed adjacent to subordinates than when housed apart.
The litter size and mass of subordinates remained unchanged, although subordinates spent significantly more time with their
pups when housed close to dominants than when housed apart; time spent with pups by dominants remained unchanged. Moreover,
females modified their reproductive output and behaviour in relation to the dominance status of their neighbours. Following
prolonged association, dominants still had greater reproductive success, but now, the time spent with pups decreased in subordinates.
We suggest that dominants adopt a strategy to increase the reproductive value of their litter, whereas subordinates adopt
a pup defence strategy. These strategies are flexible and are influenced by the dominance status and period of association
between neighbours, so that females could maximize their fitness in response to varying social conditions. 相似文献
6.
New birds on the block: new neighbors increase defensive costs for territorial male willow ptarmigan 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
Previous researchers have hypothesized that site-faithful animals may benefit from the presence of familiar neighbors. This study compares the relative costs of territorial defense against new and former neighbors by male willow ptarmigan (Lagopus lagopus). Territorial defense against new neighbors appeared to require a greater expenditure of both time and effort than did defense against former neighbors. Territorial males that had several new neighbors spent a higher proportion of time fighting than did males with fewer new neighbors, and males with both new and former neighbors spent a greater amount of time fighting with their new neighbors, on average, than with their former neighbors. In addition, fights with new neighbors occurred relatively more frequently and were longer than fights with former neighbors. Finally, fights involving new neighbors tended to escalate to higher levels than fights between former neighbors. Reduced defensive costs for site-faithful, territorial males may provide one explanation for the tendency of males to be more site-faithful than females in many species. 相似文献
7.
Gabriele Gerlach 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1996,39(3):159-170
Emigration in small mammals may be strongly related to social factors, but direct observations of emigrants are rare. Feral
house mice (Mus domesticus) were studied using a population cage system that allowed continuous observation of individually marked animals. Mice that
left their natal cage and took up residence in cages that could only be reached by crossing a water barrier were defined as
emigrants. Six pairs of house mice with their litters were placed in the system, and data on aggressive interactions, body
weight, reproduction, mortality and emigration were collected daily. Both sexes emigrated, but males did so twice as often
as females. Population density was not correlated with the frequency of aggression, and had no effect on the weight of emigrating
individuals. Male emigrants suffered more aggression before emigration than their non-emigrant brothers of the same age; they
were aggressively driven out by other males, predominantly by the father. Female emigration depended on the female’s chances
of reproduction. The probability of a female reproducing decreased with increasing birth order. Females born in a late litter,
who therefore had only a low chance of reproduction, dispersed earlier than those of early litters. Resident males were reproductively
suppressed. Male offspring had two different strategies for attaining top rank. They could develop rapidly and reach sexual
maturity early on, but face competition with the father, risking being forced to emigrate. Alternatively, they could develop
slowly, stay within their family and wait for a chance to take over the dominant position. It is concluded that emigration
in male and female feral house mice is caused by intrasexual competition.
Received: 13 July 1995/Accepted after revision: 8 June 1996 相似文献
8.
Residents of mating territories interact with different categories of conspecifics: females, nonneighbor males, and neighbor males. Interaction with these different types of conspecifics is likely to have different costs and benefits; for example, interactions with females will be beneficial, while interactions with neighbors are more likely to be costly. In this study, we investigated patterns of intrusions and space use in territorial male amberwing dragonflies (Perithemis tenera) to test the idea that residents will adjust their use of space to maximize their beneficial interactions with conspecifics while minimizing their costly interactions with conspecifics. Because territories were arranged linearly around the edge of a pond, each resident had two neighbors, one of which was often closer to the focal resident than the other. Residents experienced more intrusions by neighbors and fewer intrusions by females on the side of their closer neighbor. Residents generally perched on the side of their territory that experienced the fewest intrusions by neighbors and the most intrusions by females, but the pattern was more strongly related to neighbor intrusions than female intrusions. Subsequent to pursuits of neighbors and females, residents tended to shift their perches away from where they pursued neighbors but toward where they pursued females. Nonneighbor intrusions were not affected by neighbor proximity, nor did residents adjust their space use in response to nonneighbor intrusions. Our results suggest that residents do adjust their space use in response to intrusions by conspecifics, that their adjustment depends on the type of conspecific that intruded, and that residents may be using a simple decision rule such as "move away from male intrusions, move closer to female intrusions" to adjust their within-territory space use.Communicated by D. Gwynne 相似文献
9.
Mitani and Rodman (1979) showed that a simple measure of the defendability of the range area could differentiate fairly successfully between territorial and non-territorial species of primates. Their analysis has, however, been much criticised on the grounds that it considered only the day journey length and the diameter of the range area. We develop a number of more realistic indices of territory defendability that take into account both the length of the boundary to be defended and the detection distance as well as linear measures of range size. These indices (a) discriminate between territorial and non-territorial species more successfully than the Mitani-Rodman index and (b) allow the results to be extended to species which do not forage in cohesive groups. However, our results suggest that the Mitani-Rodman analysis was in many ways correct. 相似文献
10.
The relative roles of kinship and familiarity in affecting an individual’s growth and fitness are not easy to disentangle.
Not only is an individual more likely to have prior behavioral interactions with conspecifics in close proximity, it may also
be related (in terms of kinship) to those nearby conspecifics. While some studies have inferred that kin discrimination affects
fitness correlates, other studies found that familiarity alone can reduce aggressive interactions, thus increasing fitness.
These studies have all focused on intra-age class pairs or groups. However, many animals interact with conspecifics from different
cohorts. In many populations, adults of Plethodon cinereus territorially defend rocks and logs that retain moisture and food resources. We investigated whether juveniles of P. cinereus grew more in the presence of adults that were relatives or familiar. We collected pairs of juveniles and adults found under
the same cover objects in the forest (familiar) and pairs of juveniles and adults found under different cover objects, approximately
10 m apart (unfamiliar). We determined parentage and relatedness of the adult–juvenile pairs and then placed these pairs in
semi-natural mesocosms for 17 days. We found that juveniles housed with familiar adults had significantly greater increases
in mass and snout–vent length than juveniles housed with unfamiliar adults in 2006 but not in 2007. Relatedness had no effect
on growth. In addition, juveniles cohabitating with adults were not more likely to be their offspring. At least under certain
environmental conditions, familiarity with adults, independent of relatedness or parentage, increased the growth of juvenile
salamanders. 相似文献
11.
A few species of the weakly electric snoutfish, the African freshwater family Mormyridae, have been reported to vocalise. However, allopatric populations of a single species were never compared. Members of three allopatric Marcusenius macrolepidotus populations, originating from the Upper Zambezi River in Namibia, the Buzi River (Mozambique), and the Incomati River system in South Africa, vocalised with pulsatile growl- and tonal hoot sounds in dyadic confrontation experiments. A high rate of growling accompanied territorial and agonistic interactions and also non-threatening interactions between males and females, which in one pair appeared to be courtship. Growl sound characteristics of M. macrolepidotus from the Incomati system differed from those of the Upper Zambezi in a significantly higher frequency of the first harmonic (mean, 355 Hz vs 266 Hz). The two vocalising males from the Buzi River generated growls about twice as long as the other fish. Furthermore, the growl pulse period was about 4 ms in M. macrolepidotus from the Upper Zambezi River and from the Incomati system, but 6 ms in M. macrolepidotus from the Buzi River. Hoots were only observed in agonistic encounters. Hoot oscillograms showed a sinusoidal waveform, and the mean duration of this sound was similar in Incomati system fish (mean, 161 ms), Upper Zambezi fish (172 ms) and Buzi fish (103 and 145 ms for the two vocalising individuals). The mean frequency of the first hoot harmonic was higher in Incomati system fish (326 Hz) than in Upper Zambezi fish (245 Hz). Both growl and hoot occurred only in the presence of conspecifics, probably signalling the presence and condition of an opponent, territory owner or potential mate. This is the first evidence for (1) sound production and acoustical communication in another species and genus, M. macrolepidotus, from southern Africa to be (2) geographically differentiated. 相似文献
12.
How animals divide space can have fundamental implications for the population dynamics of territorial species. It has recently been proposed that space can be divided if animals tend to avoid fight locations, rather than the winner of fights gaining access to exclusive resources, behaviour that generates exclusive territories in two-dimensional space. A game-theory model has shown that this avoidance behaviour can be adaptive, but the adaptiveness has not been investigated in a spatially realistic context. We present a model that investigates potential strategies for the acquisition of territories when two-dimensional space must be divided between individuals. We examine whether exclusive territories form when animals avoid all encounters with others, or only those encounters that have led to losing fights, under different fighting costs and population densities. Our model suggests that when fighting costs are high, and the population density is low, the most adaptive behaviour is to avoid fight locations, which generates well-defined, exclusive territories in a population that is able to resist invasion by more aggressive strategies. Low fighting costs and high population densities lead to the break-down of territoriality and the formation of large, overlapping home ranges. We also provide a novel reason as to why so-called paradoxical strategies do not exist in nature: if we define a paradoxical strategy as an exact mirror-image of a common-sense one, it must respond in the opposite way to a draw as well as to wins and losses. When this is the case, and draws are common (fight outcomes are often not clear-cut in nature), the common-sense strategy is more often adaptive than a paradoxical alternative.Communicated by P. Bednekoff 相似文献
13.
We studied movement and site fidelity of males and females of the territorial frog Allobates femoralis (Aromobatidae) in a population in the Nature Reserve “Les Nouragues” in French Guiana, South America. Observations during
3 months in 2006 ascertained intra-seasonal site fidelity for males and females. Males actively defend large multi-purpose
territories whereas females retreat to small resting sites from where they commute to neighbouring males for courtship and
mating. Female short-term movement corroborates the previous assumption of a polygynous or promiscuous resource-defence mating
system. Year-to-year recaptures from 2005 until 2008 revealed distinct patterns of inter-annual movement for males and regional
site fidelity for females. Males abandon their territories and have to re-negotiate them when reproduction starts again at
the end of the dry season. Females are not subject to intra- or inter-sexual territorial competition and as a result move
significantly less between reproductive seasons than males. Male long-term movement reflects spatial structure and prevailing
social interactions and is a reliable indicator for tadpole deposition sites. The combined effects of intra- and inter-seasonal
movement promote the diversity of mates for both sexes. 相似文献
14.
15.
Increased energy expenditure due to increased territorial defense in male lizards after phenotypic manipulation 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
Catherine A. Marler Glenn Walsberg Myra L. White Michael Moore C. A. Marler 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1995,37(4):225-231
Fitness tradeoffs are difficult to examine because many fitness variables are correlated and vary in the same direction. Phenotypic manipulation circumvents many of these difficulties, and here we used this technique to examine mechanisms for tradeoffs between increased aggression (territorial defense) and survivorship. The behavioral phenotype of male mountain spiny lizards (Sceloporus jarrovi) was manipulated with testosterone to increase territorial defense, a sexually selected trait. We previously demonstrated that increased territorial defense results in a decrease in survival caused by a lower ratio of energy intake to energy expenditure. Here we measured energy consumption of increased territorial aggression using the doubly labeled water technique in the field and compared males with and without testosterone implants (Fig. 1). In a supplementary study we measured standard metabolic rate using captive lizards given similar testosterone implants to examine if an increase in energy expenditure was a result of only an increase in standard metabolic rate (Fig. 3). Our results indicated that a primary contribution to tradeoffs between increased territorial defense and survivorship could be made by a 31% increase in energy expenditure in the field that is not due to an increase in standard metabolic rate. 相似文献
16.
How do territorial animals gain ownership of an area? Early modelling has considered the evolution of fighting when the winner can claim the right to the resource. Recently, alternative hypotheses have been offered where repeated interactions lead to division of space through 'nagging' instead of one decisive fight. However, these models assume that animals avoid areas in which they have taken part in aggressive interactions, but do not consider whether avoidance itself is adaptive. We aim to bridge this gap between mechanistic and adaptive explanations, by presenting a game-theory model where individuals choose whether to return to an area after a fight with a specific outcome (win, loss, draw). We show that avoidance of areas where fights have occurred can be adaptive, but only if benefits of access to the area are low compared to costs of fighting. Otherwise, one individual (typically the winner) responds by returning to the area, and the other (loser) avoids it. In such cases, space is gained by winning fights. We also consider the role of conventions. If responses to fights were purely conventional, paradoxical strategies where losers of fights gain ownership would be equally logical as common-sense ones where winners claim ownership. Paradoxical solutions can be stable but only when there is little difference in fighting ability between the competitors, when individuals adhere very strictly to a behavioural rule without much random variation, and when the population in its ancient state used a paradoxical strategy. 相似文献
17.
When to be neighbourly: differential agonistic responses in the lizard Platysaurus broadleyi 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Martin J. Whiting 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1999,46(3):210-214
Differential treatment of neighbours and strangers (the dear enemy phenomenon) can reduce the costly effects of territory
defence. Individual recognition, and by extension the dear enemy phenomenon, was examined in the cordylid lizard Platysaurus broadleyi. I had no knowledge of familiarity between individuals and therefore classed intruders as neighbours or non-neighbours and
tested for differences in agonistic response consistent with the dear enemy phenomenon. In the dear enemy phenomenon, levels
of aggression are inversely related to degree of familiarity such that residents are more tolerant of immediate neighbours
than distant territory holders. A manipulative field experiment in conjunction with field observations of known territory
holders revealed that resident males allowed neighbours to approach more closely than non-neighbours before challenging them,
residents were more aggressive towards non-neighbours than neighbours, contests between neighbours were significantly longer
than between neighbours and non-neighbours and contests between residents and non-neighbours were likely to result in a win
for the resident, while neighbours frequently drew contests. These results suggest a level of recognition consistent with
the dear enemy phenomenon. I also manipulated front leg colour to test the hypothesis that it alone could serve as a cue for
individual recognition. I found no support for this hypothesis and suggest that multiple cues operate in individual recognition.
Received: 4 March 1999 / Received in revised form: 11 April 1999 / Accepted: 19 April 1999 相似文献
18.
Spatial and behavioral interactions between a native and introduced salamander species 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
L. J. Rissler Amy M. Barber Henry M. Wilbur A. M. Baker 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2000,48(1):61-68
Behavioral interactions with native species may influence the invasiveness of introduced species. The salamanders Plethodon glutinosus and P. jordani in the eastern United States share many life history traits and demonstrate complex interspecific interactions that range
geographically from competitive exclusion to sympatry. P. jordani was introduced to Mountain Lake Biological Station, Virginia, USA, between the years 1935 and 1945. We tested whether competition
for space may influence the invasion of P. jordani into native P. glutinosus habitat by utilizing data from natural distributions, a field experiment, and controlled laboratory experiments. No environmental
variables differed where P. glutinosus and P. jordani were collected in the field at the site of P. jordani introduction. In the field experiment, P. glutinosus was more fully exposed during foraging bouts in cages shared with heterospecifics as opposed to ones shared with conspecific
salamanders. Condition (mass relative to body length) of salamanders at the end of the 3 months did not differ between conspecific
and heterospecific treatments. In the laboratory, P. glutinosus most often attained the single burrow in the arena, but residency status had no effect. Species cohabited the burrow 50%
of the time. Pair-wise encounters in the laboratory indicated that both species spend less than 20% of the time in aggressive
behaviors as juveniles. Adults showed no behavior interpreted by us as aggression during pair-wise encounters.
Received: 19 December 1999 / Accepted: 18 March 2000 相似文献
19.
Common shrews (Sorex araneus) maintain a foraging territory for most of their immature life. Possessing a high-quality territory is vital for overwinter
survival in the harsh boreal climate, and hence, competitive ability in territorial disputes is expected to be an important
component of individual fitness. To test possible association between individual inbreeding and fitness, we used neutral arena
trials to assess the competitive performance of young common shrews. The experiment involved pairs of individuals originating
from small island populations, where breeding must often occur between related individuals, and from large outbred mainland
populations. The percentage of neutral arena tests that an individual won was highly significantly explained by internal relatedness,
a surrogate measure of individual inbreeding, measured using ten microsatellite markers. Body size, sex, learning, and population
type (mainland vs island) made no significant contributions. Even a low level of individual inbreeding may lead to significant
adverse consequences in multiple territorial contests, which may represent a significant cause of inbreeding depression in
many wild vertebrate populations. 相似文献
20.
We tested several hypotheses to explain low between-year territory fidelity in a breeding population of yellow-headed blackbirds (Xanthocephalus xanthocephalus). During a 5-year study the population of territorial males declined by two-thirds and some of the marshes that supported territories significantly deteriorated. Individual males held territories and bred for an average of 1.9 years. Of males that bred for at least 2 years, 30% skipped owning a territory in the study area during at least 1 year of their breeding lifetimes. Our information suggests that they may have bred outside of the area in those years. Of males with territories in two or more breeding seasons, 60% changed breeding marshes at least once. Males changed territories during 42.9% of between-year opportunities to do so. We found no support for the hypotheses that male yellow-headed blackbirds: (1) are more likely to move when territory density is low; (2) are likely to abandon territories that are deteriorating; or (3) change territories to improve their reproductive success. We suggest three non-mutually exclusive explanations for the yellow-headed blackbird's weak site fidelity: (1) it is a response to habitat deterioration and to other factors that may be causing the population's decline; (2) the males, being migratory, make fresh settlement decisions each year after they arrive on the breeding grounds in the general vicinity of their previous year's breeding; (3) yellow-headed blackbirds may have evolved in, and be adapted to, highly unstable habitats, moving frequently in response to changes in local breeding site conditions.
Correspondence to: L.D. Beletsky 相似文献