共查询到19条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
2.
M. Polak Larry L. Wolf William T. Starmer J. S. F. Barker 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2001,49(2-3):196-205
The mating plug in Drosophila hibisci Bock is a firm, gelatinous structure that forms within the female’s uterus during copulation. Two non-mutually exclusive
hypotheses for the function of the plug were evaluated. The plug may serve as a nutritional gift that females digest, using
the constituents for somatic maintenance or to provision eggs as they mature within the ovaries. Alternatively, the plug may
act as a chastity enforcement device by preventing subsequent copulations, and thereby reducing sperm competition. Plug size
did not decrease within females over a period of 2 days, and dietary treatment in females did not affect plug size. The extent
of ovarian provisioning was also not related to plug size. These results weaken the nutritional gift hypothesis. In contrast,
the probability of a second copulation increased sharply with an experimental decrease in plug size. Moreover, females with
plugs experimentally reduced in size were courted significantly more and mated significantly faster than females with larger
plugs. These results support the chastity enforcement hypothesis. The plug retains the ejaculate and concentrates sperm at
the anterior end of the uterus near the apertures of the sperm storage organs. The presence of the plug thus probably facilitates
the movement of sperm into storage by retaining sperm at the anterior end of the uterus near the apertures of the sperm storage
organs, which may be especially important for D. hibisci, in which sperm length is nearly twofold greater than ventral receptacle length. Matings with newly eclosed virgin females
were significantly shorter than with older virgins, and copulations with the younger virgins ended more often without any
sperm having yet entered into storage. The effectiveness of the plug in safeguarding a male’s ejaculate may have favoured
the evolution of shortened copula durations with young virgins. One fitness advantage of shortened copula duration could be
time liberated for the pursuit of further mating opportunities.
Received: 12 May 2000 / Revised: 22 September 2000 / Accepted: 15 October 2000 相似文献
3.
We used data from a long-term study (15 years) of fallow deer to report for the first time the lifetime mating success, overall
variance in lifetime mating success, and age-specific mortality levels of males. Fallow bucks that gain matings have higher
social dominance rank, higher rates of fighting, and invest more in vocal display during the breeding season than unsuccessful
males. Therefore, we examined if mating was associated with trade-offs in terms of survival, lifespan, and mating potential.
We found that the variance in lifetime mating success was very high: 34 (10.7%) males mated, and of those, the 10 most successful
males gained 73% of all matings (n=934). Mortality rates were generally high and only 22.3% (71/318) of males reached social maturity, i.e., 4 years. The oldest
male was 13 years old. We found that fallow bucks that mated were not more likely to die during the following year, did not
suffer from a reduction in lifespan, and did not incur lower mating success later in life as a result of mating during the
early years of social maturity. Our results show that mating males at age 5 years (and possibly 9 years) may be more likely
to survive than non-mating males. Additionally, the number of matings gained by males during the first years of social maturity
was positively correlated with lifespan. We suggest that mating males are of higher quality than non-mating males because
they are not more likely to incur trade-offs as a result of their increased reproductive efforts.
Received: 9 November 1999 / Revised: 30 April 2000 / Accepted: 27 May 2000 相似文献
4.
Social and mating system of cooperatively breeding laughing kookaburras (Dacelo novaeguineae) 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
DNA fingerprinting was combined with field observations over four breeding seasons to investigate the social structure and
mating system of the laughing kookaburra (Dacelo novaeguineae). Groups comprised a socially dominant pair and up to six helpers of either sex. Helpers were always recruited from young
hatched in the group. Territorial inheritance, which is a feature of other cooperative breeders and an oft-cited benefit of
philopatry, did not occur. Helpers only attained dominant status in an established group by dispersing into a vacant dominant
position in that group. However, helpers could also form new groups by excising a new territory, often through a ”budding”
process. The mating system was overwhelmingly monogamous. There were no cases of extra-group parentage in a sample of 140
nestlings; within groups of three or more birds, dominance predicted parentage almost perfectly (99.2% of 129 nestlings),
irrespective of whether helpers in the group were related to one or both dominant birds. This is contrary to predictions from
models of reproductive skew, possibly because they currently fail to incorporate the willingness of females to share reproduction
among males.
Received: 15 May 1999 / Received in revised form: 2 November 1999 / Accepted: 6 November 1999 相似文献
5.
Relatedness, polyandry and extra-group paternity in the cooperatively-breeding white-browed scrubwren (Sericornis frontalis ) 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
Linda A. Whittingham Peter O. Dunn Robert D. Magrath 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,40(4):261-270
We used DNA fingerprinting to examine the genetic parentage and mating system of the cooperatively breeding white-browed
scrubwren, Sericornis frontalis, in Canberra, Australia. Our analyses revealed a remarkable variety of mating tactics and social organization. Scrubwrens
bred in pairs or multi-male groups that consisted of a female and two or more males. Females were always unrelated to the
pair male or alpha (dominant) male. Among multi-male groups we found three different mating tactics. Firstly, when alpha and
beta (subordinate) males were unrelated, they usually shared paternity in the brood. This resulted in both males gaining reproductive
benefits directly. Secondly, when beta males were not related to the female but were related to the alpha males, beta males
sired offspring in some broods. In this situation, beta males gained reproductive benefits both directly and potentially indirectly
(through the related alpha male). Thirdly, when beta males were related to the female or both the female and alpha male, they
remained on their natal territory and did not sire any offspring. Thus beta males gained only indirect reproductive benefits.
Overall, when group members were related closely, the dominant male monopolized reproductive success, whereas when the members
were not related closely the two males shared paternity equally. This positive association between monopolization of reproduction
and relatedness is predicted by models of reproductive skew, but has not been reported previously within a single population
of birds. Other cooperatively breeding birds with both closely related and unrelated helpers may show a similar variety of
mating tactics. Finally, we found that extra-group paternity was more common in pairs (24% of young) than in multi-male groups
(6%), and we discuss three possible reasons for this difference.
Received: 21 May 1996 / Accepted after revision: 14 December 1996 相似文献
6.
L. Hughes B. Siew-Woon Chang D. Wagner N. E. Pierce 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2000,47(3):119-128
The mating system of the Australian lycaenid butterfly, Jalmenus evagoras, is highly unusual compared to most other Lepidoptera. Characteristics of this system, which has been termed an ’explosive
mating strategy,’ include the formation of an intensely competitive mating aggregation of males, a highly male biased operational
sex ratio, a lack of discrimination and mate choice by both sexes, a high variance in male mating success, and female monogamy.
In this study, we tested the hypothesis that multiple mating by males imposes physiological costs resulting in smaller spermatophores,
and that this results in a fitness cost to females. We found that male J. evagoras transferred only 2.2% of their eclosion weight during their first mating, consistent with the hypothesis that males of monandrous
species produce a relatively small investment. The wet weight of the ejaculate declined by an average of 27% at the second
mating and the dry weight by 29%, and an intermating interval of 5–9 days was needed for the ejaculate to return to the size
at the first mating, regardless of male size or age. Wet ejaculate mass increased proportionally with male size, though dry
mass was proportionally larger in smaller males. Ejaculate mass tended to increase with male age at both first and second
matings. Female characteristics, in general, did not affect ejaculate mass, although the wet weight of the ejaculate was positively
associated with female weight at the second mating. Copulation duration increased from 2.4 h to approximately 3 h at the second
mating, and to over 4 h at the third and fourth matings. Fecundity was positively correlated with female size but not with
mating history, copulation duration, or any other characteristics measured for either males or females. Female longevity declined
significantly as the number of times the male partner had previously mated increased. We conclude that despite the small male
investment in ejaculate, the costs of multiple mating may nonetheless be significant, as indicated by the reduction in ejaculate
mass, an increase in copulation duration, and reduction in female lifespan with increasing mating number.
Received: 22 January 1999 / Received in revised form: 28 July 1999 / Accepted: 18 September 1999 相似文献
7.
C. Cordero 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2000,48(6):458-462
The differential costs of mating paid by males and females influence the nature and strength of sexual selection. In butterflies,
males invest a relatively large amount of time and resources in each mating, but male survival costs of mating have not been
demonstrated. I present the results of experiments designed to measure the effect of different aspects of mating on male longevity
in the polygynous butterfly Callophrys xami. In experiment 1, I compared the longevity of pairs of males that produced similar amounts of spermatophore, but that mated
at different rates, a different numbers of times, and that produced spermatophores at different rates, and found that the
longevity of ”low-mating-rate” males was not different from that of ”high-mating-rate” males. In experiment 2, the longevity
of virgin males was not significantly different from that of multiply mated males. In experiment 3, I used resource-limited
males resulting from experimental food limitation of last-instar larvae; resource-limited virgin males lived significantly
more days than resource-limited multiply mated males. Since ecological costs of mating (e.g., disease transmission, predation
risk) were excluded in the experiment, diminished male longevity was a product of physiological costs of sexual interactions.
These results suggest that the cost of ejaculate production is an important cause of longevity reduction when there are resource
limitations; however, the role of other possible physiological costs of mating in longevity reduction is still unknown.
Received: 21 March 2000 / Accepted: 26 August 2000 相似文献
8.
Allison J. Abell 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,41(4):217-226
The association between spatial proximity and paternity was studied in a population of the striped plateau lizard, Sceloporus virgatus. The relationship between estimated mating success and male phenotypic traits was examined for a sample of 55 males. DNA
samples were obtained from 13 female-offspring families. The males with the closest spatial proximity to each female were
tested as possible sires within each family. Fingerprinting with two multilocus hypervariable minisatellite probes revealed
a strong correspondence between male-female spatial proximity and actual paternity. Paternity could be assigned for 72 of
the 100 hatchlings. Most hatchlings with identifiable sires were attributed to a male with the highest category of spatial
proximity to the mother. However, there was a low to moderate level of multiple paternity within clutches, and for some clutches
probable sires could not be identified even though the most likely behavioural candidates were tested. Thus, nonterritorial
males or other males lacking strong social and spatial relationships with females may achieve some degree of reproductive
success. Analysis of mating success revealed that male success increased with body size, up to a point beyond which larger
size conferred no advantage.
Received: 7 January 1997 / Accepted after revision: 16 June 1997 相似文献
9.
Weapon size versus body size as a predictor of winning in fights between shore crabs, Carcinus maenas (L.) 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
Lynne U. Sneddon Felicity A. Huntingford Alan C. Taylor 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,41(4):237-242
Relative body size (carapace width) and weapon size (chela length) were used as indicators of resource holding potential (RHP)
in the agonistic behaviour of male shore crabs, Carcinus maenas (L.). Weapon size was found to be a more reliable predictor of the outcome of pairwise fights than body size. Crabs with
longer chelae than their opponents were more likely to win fights than crabs with relatively larger bodies. Body size had
less influence on the outcome of fights. Relative body and weapon size did not influence initiation of contests but did affect
the likelihood of winning; however, this was significant only for weapon size. Winning crabs had heavier claws with greater
surface area than losing crabs. There was no relationship between relative size and fight duration. The frequency of cheliped
display increased with chela length and win- ners performed significantly more displays than losers.
Received: 5 February 1997 / Accepted after revision: 20 May 1997 相似文献
10.
Summary. It has long been assumed that the North American pipevine swallowtail, Battus philenor (L.) (Papilionidae, Troidini), is protected from natural enemies by aristolochic acids sequestered from its Aristolochia food plants. This study confirmed that populations of B. philenor from Virginia and east Texas sequester these compounds. A comparison of the aristolochic acid profiles of the Virginia butterflies
and their A. macrophylla food plants revealed several differences. The aristolochic acid fraction of the foliage was dominated by aristolochic acids
I and II, whereas the insects had a much lower proportion of aristolochic acid II and contained, in addition, substantial
amounts of aristolochic acids Ia and IVa, which were not detected in the plants. The eggs, larval integument, osmeterial glands,
pupal cuticle, and adults (wings and bodies) all contained aristolochic acids. These findings help explain the abundant ecological
data indicating that both immature and adult B. philenor are unpalatable and protected from natural enemies.
Received 7 April 2000; accepted 31 May 2000 相似文献
11.
Divorce and extrapair mating in female black-capped chickadees (Parus atricapillus): separate strategies with a common target 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
S. M. Ramsay K. A. Otter D. J. Mennill L. M. Ratcliffe P. T. Boag 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2000,49(1):18-23
Patterns of divorce and extrapair mating can provide insights into the targets of female choice in free-living birds. In resident,
site-faithful species with continuous partnerships, the better options and the incompatibility hypotheses provide the most
likely explanations for divorce. Extrapair mating can be explained by a number of hypotheses often making similar predictions.
For example, the good genes and future partnerships hypo- theses predict similar patterns if males with good genes also make
the best future partners. By considering both divorce and extrapair mating, it may be possible to distinguish between these
comparable hypotheses. We examined natural patterns of divorce and extrapair mating in a long-term study of black-capped chickadees
(Parus atricapillus). Out of 144 partnerships over 8 years, we observed 11 divorces and 38 faithful pairs between seasons. Females usually divorced
between their first and second breeding seasons for males of higher social rank than their previous partners, had similar
reproductive success prior to divorce as females who retained their previous partners, and did not divorce on the basis of
previous reproductive success. These results confirm earlier experimental evidence that females divorce for better options.
Females who divorced were significantly more likely to have had mixed-paternity broods prior to divorce than females who stayed
with their previous partners. There was no evidence that females divorced in favour of previous extrapair partners. These
results support the good genes hypothesis for extrapair mating, suggesting that female chickadees use divorce and multiple
mating as separate strategies sharing a common target.
Received: 4 February 2000 / Revised: 20 July 2000 / Accepted: 4 September 2000 相似文献
12.
Deepa S. Pureswaran Regine Gries John H. Borden Harold D. Pierce Jr. 《Chemoecology》2000,10(4):153-168
Summary. The mountain pine beetle, Dendroctonus ponderosae Hopkins, and the pine engraver, Ips pini (Say), often co-exist in lodgepole pine, Pinus contorta var. latifolia Engelmann. Intra- and interspecific semiochemical communication occurs in both species and their complete semiochemical repertoire
and precise dynamics of pheromone production have not been elucidated. Porapak-Q extracts of captured volatiles from beetles
of each species aerated at different attack phases (freshly emerged, pioneer sex alone in the log and both sexes paired in
new galleries), followed by gas chromatographic-electroantennographic detection (GC-EAD) and GC-mass spectroscopic analyses
identified 17 compounds (seven compounds common to both species, six present in D. ponderosae and four present in I. pini) that excited the antennae of either or both species. Seven compounds for D. ponderosae and nine for I. pini had not been assessed for behavioural activity. In field trapping experiments, 2-phenylethanol produced by both species inhibited
the response of D. ponderosae to its aggregation pheromones. exo- and endo-Brevicomin produced by D. ponderosae significantly decreased the response of I. pini to its aggregation pheromone ipsdienol. Nonanal, a ubiquitous compound found in the volatiles of lodgepole pine, various
nonhosts and in both beetle species deterred the response of I. pini to ipsdienol. The occurrence of cis-verbenol, trans-verbenol and verbenone in emergent I. pini, and verbenone and 2-phenylethanol in emergent D. ponderosae suggests that these compounds may inhibit aggregation and induce dispersal following emergence. Termination of aggregation
in D. ponderosae appears to depend on the production of frontalin in combination with changes in the relative ratios of verbenone, exo-brevicomin, trans-verbenol and 2-phenylethanol. In I. pini, the cessation of ipsdienol production by males is probably the main factor in terminating aggregation.
Received 16 November 1999; accepted 7 August 2000 相似文献
13.
Post-mating odor in females of the solitary bee, Andrena nigroaenea (Apoidea, Andrenidae), inhibits male mating behavior 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
We investigated odor changes and their behavioral significance in the solitary, ground-nesting bee Andrena nigroaenea. We used gas chromatography with electroantennographic detection and performed behavioral tests with males in the field using
natural odor samples and synthetic compounds. We found that only cuticle extracts of young females elicited copulation attempts
in the males. We demonstrated that among the 17 compounds which triggered electroantennographic responses, all-trans-farnesyl hexanoate and all-trans-farnesol were significantly more abundant in unattractive cuticle extracts of A. nigroaenea females. Dufour’s gland extracts of these females also contained greater amounts of both compounds. In bioassays using synthetic
farnesyl hexanoate and farnesol we found that these compounds inhibit copulation behavior in the males. Farnesyl hexanoate
is probably synthesized in Dufour’s gland and used by females for lining brood cells. We interpret the semiochemical function
of farnesyl hexanoate and its precursor farnesol to have evolved secondarily. As an outcome of sexual selection, it facilitates
the discrimination by males of receptive females from nesting and thus already mated individuals. The dual function of these
compounds represents an elegant parsimony in the chemical communication system of this insect.
Received: 19 January 2000 / Revised: 29 May 2000 / Accepted: 24 June 2000 相似文献
14.
In monogamous species, females often choose between males according to the quality of the territories they defend, but the
extent to which females themselves contribute to territory defence is frequently underestimated. Here we test for differences
in male and female roles during paired scent-marking bouts, a key component of territorial defence, in a monogamous antelope.
In two populations (Kenya, Zimbabwe) of klipspringer, Oreotragus oreotragus, both males and females usually scent-marked at the same site, but there were significant differences between sexes in terms
of investment within bouts. Females initiated most bouts, thus dictating the marking strategy of the pair. Males initiated
relatively few bouts, but deposited more scent marks per bout than females and were usually the last to scent-mark before
leaving the site; they marked on the same branches as the female and thus overmarked her scent. Both sexes deposited more
marks during paired than solo visits. Immediately preceding and following scent-marking bouts, males approached females and
females left males more often than expected. Female scent-marking rates were higher when they were receptive than at other
times, and this increase was matched by elevated marking rates of males. Females may increase marking rates when they are
receptive in order to test the quality of their mate or to incite male competition. However, these ideas are unlikely to explain
female scent-marking behaviour outside the mating season, which appears to be related primarily to territorial defence. We
suggest that these differences in investment in scent-marking bouts are consistent with predictions that females may be autonomously
territorial and that overmarking of female scent by males is a form of mate-guarding.
Received: 17 November 1999 / Received in revised form: 24 February 2000 / Accepted: 13 March 2000 相似文献
15.
During their seasonal runs in the Okavango and other freshwater bodies in southern Africa, the sharptooth catfish, Clarias gariepinus, specialises on the bulldog, Marcusenius macrolepidotus, as its main prey. We examined whether the catfish can locate bulldogs by detecting their pulse-type electric organ discharges
(EODs). The electrosensory threshold for single-cycle, monopolar square-wave pulses was exceedingly low (down to 13 μVp–p/cm for 4-ms pulses) in trained, food-rewarded sharptooth catfish (n=4), confirming the results of Lissmann and Machin who were, however, unable to identify a biological function. Other stimulus
pulse waveforms (single-cycle, monopolar as well as bipolar sine-wave pulses) were also effective stimuli according to their
spectral low-frequency energy contents (0– 30 Hz). Male bulldogs display an EOD pulse approximately 10× the duration of female
EODs (≈0.5 ms). The C. gariepinus threshold for field-recorded playbacks of a male bulldog EOD (of long duration) was 103 μVp–p/cm, whereas the brief female and juvenile EODs were not detected (using intensities of natural EODs). EODs of other mormyrids
were detected when either monopolar or of long duration. Signal source amplitude increased linearly with standard length (SL)
in bulldogs. Signal reach, as calculated from signal source amplitude and receiver sensitivity, is up to 150 cm for a large
male bulldog (SL 27.5 cm), and 83 cm for a male that has just turned sexually mature (SL 12.6 cm). Therefore, most bulldogs
eaten by catfish are probably male, in agreement with the size distribution of bulldogs found in catfish stomachs. These results
suggest that sharptooth catfish rely heavily on their acute electrical sense during hunting, and an important function for
electroreception in an African catfish has been identified.
Received: 13 December 1999 / Revised: 20 April 2000 / Accepted: 20 May 2000 相似文献
16.
In the meerkat (Suricata suricatta), a cooperative mongoose, pups follow potential feeders while the group is foraging and emit incessant calls when soliciting
food from them. In contrast to a ’stationary’ brood of chicks, in which nestlings are fed at a fixed location, meerkat pups
are ’mobile’ and become spread out. The question arises whether meerkat pups that experience different constraints to those
facing chicks have evolved similar begging strategies. This paper describes the vocalisations that meerkat pups emit in the
context of begging and investigates the influence of these calls on food allocation by older group members and on the behaviour
of littermates. Meerkat pups use two types of calls when soliciting food from a potential feeder. The most common is a ’repeat’
call, which pups emit continuously when following an older forager over several hours a day. In addition, when a potential
feeder finds a prey item, the pups next to it emit a bout of calls with increased calling rate, amplitude and fundamental
frequency, termed ’high-pitched’ calls. Observations, together with playback experiments, showed that more prey was allocated
to pups that called longer and more intensely. The pup closest to a feeder was almost always fed. The probability of emitting
high-pitched calls did not depend on the time since a pup had received food, and the change from repeat to high-pitched calls
occurred suddenly. The main function of the high-pitched call, therefore, does not appear to be to signal a pup’s hunger state.
More likely, the two calls, in the context of begging, may be an adaptation to energetic constraints in a mobile feeding system.
Pups, which are dispersed during foraging, may emit repeat calls over long periods to prevent potential feeders from eating
all the prey themselves. At the moment a potential feeder finds prey, pups may give the more intense high-pitched calls to
direct feeders to bring the food item to them and not to a littermate. Therefore, unlike the stationary feeding system where
chicks emit one type of begging call when the feeder approaches the nest, meerkats, with a mobile feeding system, have evolved
two discrete types of vocalisations in the context of begging.
Received: 22 November 1999 / Revised: 1 July 2000 / Accepted: 17 July 2000 相似文献
17.
Antipredator behavior of a social desert rodent: footdrumming and alarm calling in the great gerbil, Rhombomys opiums 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
J. A. Randall Konstantin A. Rogovin Debra M. Shier 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2000,48(2):110-118
We sought to understand why a social, desert rodent, the great gerbil, Rhombomys opimus, expends energy and possible risk of predation by footdrumming and vocalizing in the presence of a diversity of terrestrial
predators: snakes, monitor lizards, polecats, foxes, and humans. Behavioral observations, human approaches, and experiments
with tethered predators revealed that both male and female gerbils called and footdrummed in the presence of offspring, close
relatives, and potential mates. Because adults called more often when pups were present, and solitary gerbils seldom gave
an alarm, the alarm behavior probably warns conspecifics, especially vulnerable offspring, of potential danger. We also found
that gerbils altered alarm behavior with the type of predator. They drummed more in the burrow when a dog that could not enter
the burrow was present, and they drummed more out of the burrow in response to a snake that could enter the burrow. Gerbils
vocalized and stood in an alert posture in response to all stimuli. The different footdrumming responses of gerbils to terrestrial
predators seems related to the hunting style and type of risk posed by the predator, especially its ability to enter the burrow
system.
Received: 23 August 1999 / Received in revised form: 6 December 1999 / Accepted: 25 February 2000 相似文献
18.
Jorge E. Rabinovich Martín Torres Jordá C. Bernstein 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2000,48(4):308-315
Telenomus fariai is a gregarious endoparasitoid of the eggs of several species of Triatominae (Hemiptera) with a high degree of sibmating:
males fertilize their sisters inside the host egg before emergence or emerge first and copulate with their sisters as these
emerge. Our results show that, when laying alone, T. fariai behaves adaptively, minimizing offspring mortality and conforming to the prediction of local mate competition (LMC) theory
by laying a single male, which is sufficient to fertilize all the sisters. When more than one wasp was placed with one host,
sex ratios still conformed to LMC predictions but, despite the decreasing number of eggs laid per wasp, clutch size could
not be completely adjusted to avoid mortality. This is not surprising, as superparasitism is rare in the field. Offspring
production was independent of the contacts between conspecifics but was affected by the number of mothers laying on a single
host egg. The sex of the progeny was precisely determined: a female produced one male per clutch when laying on both unparasitized
or previously parasitized hosts. On the other hand, a mother produced less daughters when superparasitizing. Under crowded
conditions, the number of eggs laid per female wasp and per host decreased as the number of mothers increased. Developmental
mortality also increased with the number of T. fariai eggs per host, determining a maximum of approximately 14 emerged adults. Host resources per individual affected male and
female adult size with similar intensity, and male adult mortality was slightly higher than that for females. These results,
and previous findings, suggest that T. fariai attains Hamiltonian sex ratios by laying one male and a variable number of females, and that the detection of chemical marks
left by conspecifics provides information on the number of foundresses sharing a patch.
Received: 4 February 2000 / Received in revised form: 19 April 2000 / Accepted: 20 May 2000 相似文献
19.
Summary. In a wind tunnel bioassay the effect of three concentrations of natural extracts of (1) Scots pine wood, Pinus sylvestris, and (2) larval frass on the behavioural response of unmated females and males of the old house borer, Hylotrupes bajulus, was tested and compared to the behavioural effects of the male-produced sex pheromone (3R)-3-hydroxy-2-hexanone. The influence on the behaviour of both sexes was found to be equally significant for the two higher
concentrated hexane extracts of wood and larval frass. Therefore several synthetic monoterpenes present in the extracts and
ethanol were tested at the two higher concentrations (1:100, 1:1000 vol/vol). Among the higher concentrated monoterpenoid
hydrocarbons [(+)-α-pinene, (+)-β-pinene, (+)-limonene], only α-pinene increased the activity, orientation towards scent source and interest towards conspecifics. The tests with higher
concentrated ethanol and the oxygenated monoterpenes [(-)-verbenone, (-)-trans-pinocarveol, (+)-terpinen-4-ol, (+)-α-terpineol, (-)-myrtenol] revealed that verbenone is the most effective stimulant for the females, followed by trans-pinocarveol, terpinen-4-ol and α-terpineol. For males, terpinen-4-ol was the only mediator significantly inducing attraction and orientation towards the scent
source combined with an interest in conspecifics apparent by fighting or courtship behaviour. Males did not respond to verbenone
which is a main compound of larval frass. Myrtenol and ethanol were ineffective in both sexes. In fact behavioural observations
suggest that the beetles were repelled by the high dose of myrtenol. Using the ten-fold lower dose of the synthetic monoterpenes
(1:1000 vol/vol), all semiochemicals except myrtenol lost activity. Myrtenol, however, induced behavioural responses, like
increased activity and orientation towards scent source, only at the low concentration. Based on the results, primary attraction
of unmated old house borer is probably mediated by monoterpenes of coniferous wood, while secondary attraction to infested
wood would occur in response to volatiles of larval frass.
Received 5 May 1999; accepted 30 September 1999 相似文献