共查询到7条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
Peter Weindler Frank Böhme Varis Liepa Wolfgang Wiltschko 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1998,42(4):289-294
To assess the role of celestial rotation during daytime in the development of the magnetic compass course, pied flycatchers
(Ficedula hypoleuca Pallas, Muscicapidae) were handraised in Latvia under various celestial and magnetic conditions. Tests were performed during
autumn migration in the local geomagnetic field (50 000 nT, 73° inclination) in the absence of celestial cues. A group of
birds that had never seen the sky showed a bimodal preference for the migratory southwest-northeast axis, whereas a second
group that had been exposed to the natural sky from sunrise to sunset in the local geomagnetic field showed a unimodal preference
for the seasonally appropriate southwesterly direction. A third group that had also been exposed to the daytime sky, but in
the absence of magnetic compass information, also oriented bimodally along a southwest-northeast axis. These findings demonstrate
that observing celestial rotation during daytime enables birds to choose the right end of the migratory axis for autumn migration
at the Latvian test location. This transformation of axial behavior into appropriate migratory orientation, however, requires
the birds to have simultaneous access to information on both celestial rotation and the geomagnetic field.
Received: 19 September 1997 / Accepted after revision: 22 November 1997 相似文献
2.
Vera A. Matrosova Ilya A. Volodin Elena V. Volodina Andrey F. Babitsky 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2007,62(2):181-191
In most mammals, larger adult body size correlates with lower fundamental frequency and more closely spaced formants in vocalizations
relative to juveniles. In alarm whistles of two free-living rodents, the speckled ground squirrel Spermophilus suslicus and yellow ground squirrel S. fulvus, these cues to body size were absent despite prominent differences in body weight and skull and larynx sizes between juveniles
and adults. No significant correlations were found between the individual maximum fundamental frequency and body weight, both
within age classes and for pooled samples of all animals within species. Furthermore, the mean alarm whistle maximum fundamental
frequencies did not differ significantly between age classes (juvenile versus adult) in the speckled squirrel and were even
significantly lower in juvenile yellow squirrels. We discuss the hypothesis that the obfuscation of vocal differences between
juvenile and adult squirrels may represent a special adaptation of pup vocal behaviour—a form of “vocal mimicry,” resulting
in imitation of adult vocal pattern to avoid infanticide and age-dependent predation risk. 相似文献
3.
Behaviorally dominant members of blue-footed booby (Sula nebouxii) broods can effect siblicide by restricting access of subordinate siblings to parents providing food. In spite of their capacity for siblicide, dominant chicks permit subordinates to feed during short-term food shortage; in fact, the proportion of the food that the dominant takes is independent of the total amount delivered in older chicks. A model of optimal food distribution suggests that dominant chicks maximize their inclusive fitness with this pattern, rather than by satisfying their own food requirements and leaving what remains for the subordinate sibling. The indirect reproductive potential represented by a chick's sibling appears to have influenced the evolution of siblicidal brood reduction in this species. 相似文献
4.
The cost of host egg damage caused by a brood parasite: experiments on great spotted cuckoos (Clamator glandarius) and magpies (Pica pica) 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Manuel Soler Juan José Soler Tomás Pérez-Contreras 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1999,46(6):381-386
Adult great spotted cuckoos, Clamator glandarius, frequently damage one or more eggs of their magpie host, Pica pica, without removing or eating them. The presence of damaged host eggs could signal parasitism thereby increasing the probability
that the parasitic egg is ejected. This hypothesis was tested by experimentally introducing a model cuckoo egg with or without
damaged host eggs. Magpie responses to experimental parasitism did not differ significantly between treatments implying that
damaged host eggs are not used by magpies to assess parasitism. We followed the fate of magpie eggs naturally damaged by the
great spotted cuckoo or experimentally damaged by us. Host response was very similar for naturally or experimentally damaged
host eggs, but varied significantly according to the type of egg damage, eggs being removed more frequently when pecked than
crushed, while cracked eggs were never removed. However, the egg damage that most readily causes egg removal is albumen leakage.
Received: 30 November 1998 / Received in revised form: 7 June 1999 / Accepted: 12 June 1999 相似文献
5.
Walter M. Farina 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1996,38(1):59-64
Dancing and trophallactic behaviour of forager honey bees, Apis mellifera ligustica >Spinola, that returned from an automatic feeder with a regulated flow rate of 50% weight-to-weight sucrose solution (range:
0.76–7.65 μl/min) were studied in an observation hive. Behavioural parameters of dancing, such as probability, duration and
dance tempo, increased with the nectar flow rate, though with very different response curves among bees. For trophallaxis
(i.e. mouth-to-mouth exchange of food), the frequency of giving-contacts and the transfer rate of the nectar increased with
the nectar flow rate. After unloading, foragers often approached other nest mates and begged for food before returning to
the food source. This behaviour was less frequent at higher nectar flow rates. These results show that the profitability of
a food source in terms of nectar flow rate had a quantitative representation in the hive through quantitative changes in trophallactic
and dancing behaviour. The role of trophallaxis as a communication channel during recruitment is discussed.
Received: 14 January 1995/Accepted after revision: 14 August 1995 相似文献
6.
Predators can have significant nonlethal effects on prey by modifying prey behaviour through chemically mediated interactions.
We examined behavioural responses of wood frog (Ranasylvatica) and American toad (Bufoamericanus) tadpoles to both direct and indirect chemical signals associated with a predatory odonate (Anaxjunius). In laboratory trials, tadpoles of both species responded strongly to water conditioned with Anax nymphs by decreasing foraging rates, becoming immobile, and moving away from the stimulus. The responses to water conditioned
with starved Anax versus Anax that fed on conspecific tadpoles did not differ significantly; these results suggest that tadpoles rely primarily on direct
signals to detect odonates. Rana did not respond to water conditioned with conspecific tissue extracts, while Bufo responded with behaviours that were indistinguishable from those of tadpoles exposed to Anax chemicals. In a field experiment, the responses of R. sylvatica tadpoles to Anax chemicals were similar to those of tadpoles observed in the laboratory. Collectively, our data indicate that tadpoles of
both species use chemical cues to assess predation risk from other community members. Tadpoles can selectively distinguish
members who pose a threat, and only evacuate food patches or reduce foraging rates when in danger. These behaviours appear
to be adaptive and are consistent with the predictions of optimality theory.
Received: 1 August 1997 / Accepted after revision: 22 November 1997 相似文献
7.
In birds, host experience can modify response to parasites but nothing similar is known for insects. We studied two desert tenebrionid beetles, the subsocial host Parastizopus armaticeps and its obligate and ubiquitous brood-(clepto)parasite Eremostibes opacus, which mimics the hosts odour spectrum. Nearly 10% of host burrows in the field (n=214) remain unparasitised even after introducing E. opacus experimentally (n=22). In discrimination tests, 7% of naive host beetles eject the parasite, showing individual variation in olfactory discrimination ability. To test for effects of prior breeding experience, naive (first time) or experienced (second time) breeders with or without prior parasite contact were confined with E. opacus at breeding onset, their behaviour recorded and parasite number counted on breeding completion. More first- than second-time breeding pairs completely excluded the parasite and second-time breeders with no prior contact with E. opacus had fewer parasites than any others. These pairs increased burrow-guarding dramatically, mostly during early reproduction, this being the best predictor of parasite number. Prolonged guarding, however, must be traded-off against other brood-care behaviours. Hence, it might pay breeding pairs to take the risk of accepting some parasites breeding within their burrow, and we expect this effect will be more pronounced if poor feeding and digging conditions are taken into account. This might explain why apparently 90% of the P. armaticeps breeding burrows in the field contain at least one parasitic E. opacus.Communicated by D. Gwynne 相似文献