共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
In the stream-dwelling isopod, Lirceus fontinalis, mating contests between males and females occur prior to pair formation. We examined the relative contribution of male preference
and female resistance to contest outcomes. We first quantified male and female behavior during typical mating interactions
and examined the relationship between time until molt (TTM) and mating outcomes. We then examined the role of male preference
and female resistance in determining mating outcomes when females differed in molt type (growth, egg deposition) and appeared
to differ in TTM (due to hormone applications). Both male preference and female resistance contributed to different components
of the mating sequence but female resistance ultimately determined whether or not pair formation occurred. Males expressed
a preference for females that appeared to be close to molt, using variation in levels of molt hormone as a cue. However, males
did not discriminate between females based on molt type.
Received: 5 March 1999 / Received in revised form: 10 August 1999 / Accepted: 16 October 1999 相似文献
2.
Environmental and genetic determinants of the male forceps length dimorphism in the European earwig Forficula auricularia L. 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
J. L. Tomkins 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1999,47(1-2):1-8
Male dimorphisms are particularly conspicuous examples of alternative reproductive strategies. The male forceps length dimorphism
in the European earwig Forficula auricularia has long been considered an example of a status- (body size) dependent male dimorphism. In this paper, I test three hypotheses
relating to the dimorphism of F. auricularia. First, that the dimorphism is status dependent and determined by nutrition. Second, that the dimorphism is a density-dependent
adaptation. Third, that there is a genetic basis to population differences in morph frequency seen in the field. These hypotheses
were tested by rearing two populations in a split-family rearing design with two diets and two densities. Populations of male
earwigs reared in the common garden differed in forceps length and relative forceps length. The populations also differed
in the morph frequencies, with 40 versus 26% long-forceped males. These results confirm the notion that there is a genotype-by-environment
interaction that determines the morph frequency in a population. There were only minor effects of density on male forceps
length and no influence of density on the male dimorphism. In accordance with the hypothesis that the morphs are status-dependent
alternatives, large-forceped males only arose on the high-protein diet that produced earwigs of a large body size. However,
not all large males produced the long-forceped phenotype. I put forward an extension of the status-dependent dimorphism model
that may account for the pattern of forceps dimorphism in this species.
Received: 18 November 1998 / Received in revised form: 14 May 1999 / Accepted: 25 July 1999 相似文献
3.
The evolution of male breeding aggregations is difficult to explain because males may reduce their reproductive success by
associating with their closest competitors. We examined aggregative behavior by male New Mexico spadefoot toads, Spea multiplicata, which form breeding choruses in rain-filled pools. We specifically asked whether males are attracted to conspecific calls
and, if so, whether they preferentially associate with those male calls that are also attractive to females. Field observations
revealed that males showed significant clustering with conspecifics within breeding ponds, whereas laboratory phonotaxis experiments
revealed that males preferentially associated with conspecific male calls. Moreover, when males were presented with conspecific
calls that differed in call rate, smaller males associated with the stimulus preferred by females (average call rate). Thus,
males appear to evaluate the attractiveness of competitors using the same trait employed by females to assess potential mates,
and males adjust their positions relative to competitors depending on their size. We discuss these results in the light of
several current hypotheses on the adaptive significance of male breeding aggregations.
Received: 20 December 1999 / Accepted: 18 March 2000 相似文献
4.
Maternal investment in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta): reproductive costs and consequences of raising sons 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Maternal investment in offspring is expected to vary according to offspring sex when the reproductive success of the progeny
is a function of differential levels of parental expenditure. We conducted a longitudinal investigation of rhesus macaques
to determine whether variation in male progeny production, measured with both DNA fingerprinting and short tandem repeat marker
typing, could be traced back to patterns of maternal investment. Males weigh significantly more than females at birth, despite
an absence of sex differences in gestation length. Size dimorphism increases during infancy, with maternal rank associated
with son’s, but not daughter’s, weight at the end of the period of maternal investment. Son’s, but not daughter’s, weight
at 1 year of age is significantly correlated with adult weight, and male, but not female, weight accounts for a portion of
the variance in reproductive success. Variance in annual offspring output was three- to fourfold higher in males than in females.
We suggest that energetic costs of rearing sons could be buffered by fetal delivery of testosterone to the mother, which is
aromatized to estrogen and fosters fat accumulation during gestation. We conclude that maternal investment is only slightly
greater in sons than in daughters, with mothers endowing sons with extra resources because son, but not daughter, mass has
ramifications for offspring sirehood. However, male reproductive tactics supersede maternal investment patterns as fundamental
regulators of male fitness.
Received: 23 July 1999 / Received in revised form: 23 February 2000 / Accepted: 13 March 2000 相似文献
5.
Call matching in the quacking frog (Crinia georgiana) 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
H. C. Gerhardt J. D. Roberts M. A. Bee J. J. Schwartz 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2000,48(3):243-251
Males of the quacking frog Crinia georgiana produce calls consisting of 1–11 notes. Playback experiments using synthetic calls showed that males tend to match the number
of notes in 2-note and 4-note stimuli; however, males tended to produce more than 1 note in response to a 1-note stimulus
and fewer than 8 notes in response to an 8-note stimulus. Successive playbacks of two, 4-note calls from separate speakers
indicate that males are likely to match the combined number of notes in the calls of two neighbors, even if they are not equidistant
from the focal male. The results are compared with the few other studies of matching in anurans, and interpreted in terms
of hypotheses developed to explain matching in songbirds. One attractive and testable hypothesis for call matching in C. georgiana is that males are attempting to produce calls that are at least as attractive to females as those of rivals, without wasting
energy.
Received: 14 February 2000 / Received in revised form: 11 April 2000 / Accepted: 3 May 2000 相似文献
6.
The small cichlid fish Neolamprologus multifasciatus is endemic to Lake Tanganyika and is unique among fish because it lives in complex social groups with several reproductive
males and females, all of which participate in defending their territory against neighbors and intruders. Individuals use
empty snail shells for breeding and shelter. Previous parentage analysis using microsatellites suggested occasional exchange
of individuals between groups. In field experiments, we found that females showed a higher tendency than males to migrate
into territories already occupied by a resident pair. The phenomenon and causes of female-biased immigration were further
investigated in aquarium experiments: Nine of 15 females, but only 2 of 15 equally sized males, settled in territories of
established pairs. Territorial males exhibited more aggression toward strange males (potential reproductive competitors) than
toward strange females (potential additional mates); their females were more aggressive toward strange females (probably competitors
for shells) than toward strange males. Apparently, a conflict exists between the sexes regarding the immigration of additional
females. This conflict seems to be the selective basis for observed active male interference in aggressive disputes between
females in the territory. Interfemale tolerance in a group was greater in the male’s presence than in his absence.
Received: 1 February 2000 / Received in revised form: 2 May 2000 / Accepted: 5 May 2000 相似文献
7.
This study used both correlative and experimental video playback methods to test the hypothesis that the secondary sexual
traits of male wolf spiders act to increase the efficacy of visual courtship displays. Direct observations of courtship of
several lycosid genera and a review of the literature revealed a significant association between ornamentation and visual
courtship displays. This suggests that the ornamentation may be playing the role of amplifier for a visual display. To test
this hypothesis, male courtship behaviors of four Schizocosa species were experimentally manipulated using video-imaging techniques. Females of species with non-visually displaying,
non-ornamented males (Schizocosa duplex and S. uetzi) did not increase in frequency of receptivity when tufts were added to conspecific males. In a species with a visual display
and foreleg pigmentation (S. stridulans), the addition of foreleg tufts increased female receptivity. In a tufted species (S. crassipes), females tended to decrease their receptivity when male ornamentation was completely removed. In visually displaying species,
ornamentation acts to increase female receptivity, supporting its role as an amplifier of a visual display.
Received: 29 December 1997 / Received in revised form: 23 October 1999 / Accepted: 13 December 1999 相似文献
8.
This study evaluated the effects of time of day, ambient temperature, and relative humidity on mother-infant interactions
in captive Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata). Unlike time of day and relative humidity, temperature influenced mother-infant interactions. Even though total time spent
in ventroventral contact did not change, lower temperatures were associated with greater attempts by the infants to maintain
contact with their mothers and with more frequent maternal rejection. Effects of temperature on mother-infant interactions
were not mediated by the effects of temperature on mothers' general activity. These results are interpreted in light of the
different thermoregulatory needs of mothers and infants, and highlight a previously neglected cause for mother-infant conflict.
Received: 23 June 1997 / Accepted after revision: 28 March 1998 相似文献
9.
Are nest characters of importance when choosing a male in the fifteen-spined stickleback (Spinachia spinachia)? 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
S. Östlund-Nilsson 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2000,48(3):229-235
Male fifteen-spined sticklebacks (Spinachia spinachia) build their nests by wrapping epiphytic algae around macroalgae and securing them with secretional threads. In this study,
I show that female fifteen-spined stickleback prefer males which have built their nests above the surrounding vegetation.
High-located nests are safer, because they are less likely to attract egg-predators such as shore crabs (Carcinus maenas). Thus, I found that shore crabs more rapidly find eggs in nests built close to the bottom than in nests well above the it.
Moreover, male-male competition could be an additional explanation as to why males build nests high up. Thus, my field results
suggest that the closer the males were to a neighbour, the higher they built. Larger males were also found to have larger
territories but male size did not correlate with the height of the nest above the surrounding vegetation. Females showed no
preference for nest size, regardless of the presence of egg-predators. However, they tended to choose nests that had no egg-predators
in the vicinity over nests with egg-predators close by. Female choosiness for certain nest characters may provide one explanation
for the evolution of male care in this species.
Received: 29 December / 1999 Revised: 18 April 2000 / Accepted: 28 May 2000 相似文献
10.
C. Cordero 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2000,48(6):458-462
The differential costs of mating paid by males and females influence the nature and strength of sexual selection. In butterflies,
males invest a relatively large amount of time and resources in each mating, but male survival costs of mating have not been
demonstrated. I present the results of experiments designed to measure the effect of different aspects of mating on male longevity
in the polygynous butterfly Callophrys xami. In experiment 1, I compared the longevity of pairs of males that produced similar amounts of spermatophore, but that mated
at different rates, a different numbers of times, and that produced spermatophores at different rates, and found that the
longevity of ”low-mating-rate” males was not different from that of ”high-mating-rate” males. In experiment 2, the longevity
of virgin males was not significantly different from that of multiply mated males. In experiment 3, I used resource-limited
males resulting from experimental food limitation of last-instar larvae; resource-limited virgin males lived significantly
more days than resource-limited multiply mated males. Since ecological costs of mating (e.g., disease transmission, predation
risk) were excluded in the experiment, diminished male longevity was a product of physiological costs of sexual interactions.
These results suggest that the cost of ejaculate production is an important cause of longevity reduction when there are resource
limitations; however, the role of other possible physiological costs of mating in longevity reduction is still unknown.
Received: 21 March 2000 / Accepted: 26 August 2000 相似文献
11.
Fisher’s 1930 theory of sex allocation predicts a population-wide 1:1 ratio of parental investment. We tested this prediction in the European beewolf, a sphecid wasp that hunts for honeybees as larval food. Because the method to quantify parental investment is of crucial importance, we compared the suitability of several different investment measures. Female/male cost ratios were determined from a sample and the total investment in sons and daughters was calculated. In addition, the actual number of prey items for sons and daughters was directly determined by excavating nests and counting the cuticle remains of the prey. Though mortality was high (70%), it had only a weak effect on the estimate of the investment ratio. Based on commonly used measures like fresh and dry weight of emerged adults, the investment ratio did not deviate from Fisher’s prediction of equal investment. However, progeny weight considerably underestimates investment in males and investment in large progeny. Measures that reflect the allocation of resources more directly (amount of provisions, brood cell volume) revealed a significant male bias and thus contradicted Fisher’s theory. Three kinds of explanation are discussed. First, non-adaptive explanations are unlikely. Second, from the spectrum of alternative adaptive theories, only models that assume a non-linear relationship between amount of investment and progeny fitness seem to be relevant for the study species. Third, though the number of prey in a brood cell seems to be a rather good measure of parental investment in European beewolves, some problems in measuring parental investment remain. These problems are of broad significance. Received: 17 June 1999 / Received in revised form: 6 July 1999 / Accepted: 11 July 1999 相似文献
12.
Sexual conflict in the snake den 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
Red-sided garter snakes (Thamnophis sirtalis parietalis) court and mate in spring, soon after they emerge from large communal overwintering dens in south-central Manitoba. Because
of a massive bias in the operational sex ratio, every female attracts intense courtship from dozens to hundreds of males.
We suggest that this courtship constitutes significant ”harassment,” because it delays the females’ dispersal from the den
and hence increases their vulnerability to predation. Small females may face the greatest costs, because they are less able
to escape from amorous males (who court all females, even juvenile animals). Our measurements show that males are stronger
and faster than females. Experimental trials confirm that the locomotor ability of females (especially small females) is greatly
reduced by the weight of a courting male. Arena trials show that intense courtship stimulates females to attempt to escape.
Remarkably, some females that are too small to produce offspring may nonetheless copulate. This precocious sexual receptivity
may benefit juvenile females because copulation renders them unattractive to males, and thus allows them to escape more easily
from the den. Female ”tactics” to escape male harassment may explain other puzzling aspects of garter snake biology including
size-assortative mating, temporal patterns in dispersal from the den, avoidance of communal dens by young-of-the-year snakes,
and female mimicry. Hence, sexual conflict may have influenced important features of the mating system and behavioral ecology
of these animals.
Received: 8 May 2000 / Revised: 28 July 2000 / Accepted: 30 July 2000 相似文献
13.
Brood sex ratios, female harem status and resources for nestling provisioning in the great reed warbler (Acrocephalus arundinaceus) 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
H. Westerdahl Staffan Bensch Bengt Hansson Dennis Hasselquist Torbjörn von Schantz 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2000,47(5):312-318
The theory of parental investment and brood sex ratio manipulation predicts that parents should invest in the more costly
sex during conditions when resources are abundant. In the polygynous great reed warbler, Acrocephalus arundinaceus, females of primary harem status have more resources for nestling provisioning than secondary females, because polygynous
males predominantly assist the primary female whereas the secondary female has to feed her young alone. Sons weigh significantly
more than daughters, and are hence likely to be the more costly sex. In the present study, we measured the brood sex ratio
when the chicks were 9 days old, i.e. the fledging sex ratio. As expected from theory, we found that female great reed warblers
of primary status had a higher proportion of sons in their broods than females of lower (secondary) harem status. This pattern
is in accordance with the results from two other species of marsh-nesting polygynous birds, the oriental reed warbler, Acrocephalus orientalis, and the yellow-headed blackbird Xanthocephalus xanthocephalus. As in the oriental reed warbler, we found that great reed warbler males increased their share of parental care as the proportion
of sons in the brood increased. We did not find any difference in fitness of sons and daughters raised in primary and secondary
nests. The occurrence of adaptive sex ratio manipulations in birds has been questioned, and it is therefore important that
three studies of polygynous bird species, including our own, have demonstrated the same pattern of a male-biased offspring
sex ratio in primary compared with secondary nests.
Received: 1 June 1999 / Received in revised form: 10 January 2000 / Accepted: 12 February 2000 相似文献
14.
Females in several sexually dimorphic species with conventional sex roles possess ornamental traits that resemble those found
in males. The evolution of such traits, however, is still poorly understood. Bluethroats (Luscinia s. svecica) are socially monogamous, sexually dichromatic passerine birds, in which female throat patch coloration varies from near
absence to near full expression of male-like coloration. A recent study, demonstrating that male bluethroats prefer colourful
females, suggests that female coloration is subject to sexual selection through male choice. However, the benefits males may
gain from mating with colourful females have not yet been identified. In this study we tested the hypothesis that female coloration
signals parental quality (the good-parent hypothesis). During the course of the same day, we recorded female care both in
the presence and the absence of the male mate. The latter was done to eliminate the confounding effect of variable male care
by removing the male temporarily. Female coloration did not correlate with female feeding rates either in the presence or
in the absence of the male. Female feeding rates in the absence and the presence of the male were positively, although weakly,
correlated. Female coloration did not correlate with female ability to compensate for the loss of male care, or with the change
in brood mass during male removal. Therefore, there is no evidence for the good-parent hypothesis to explain female plumage
coloration in bluethroats.
Received: 4 March 1999 / Received in revised form: 14 October 1999 / Accepted: 23 October 1999 相似文献
15.
Ellen J. Censky 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,40(4):221-225
Mate choice by females has been documented in a variety of taxa. Female mate choice in species lacking male resource control
or paternal care might occur if preferred males provide protection from harassment. Female mate choice was investigated in
a natural population of the non-territorial lizard Ameiva plei (Teiidae). Consort pairs were allowed to form naturally. Consort males were significantly larger than non-consort males.
After removal of consort males, the “abandoned” female's reaction to the first male who approached her was recorded. Females
rejected all small males. Female preference for large males was significantly higher than preference for small males. Large
males may be better equipped to guard the females from harassment and behavior of large males is less harassing than behavior
of small males, thereby affording the female increased foraging time.
Received: 21 June 1996 / Accepted after revision: 28 December 1996 相似文献
16.
Variation of the amplification effect of burrows of the leptodactylid frog Eupsophus emiliopugini on conspecific calls generated externally was investigated. Advertisement calls broadcast through a loudspeaker placed in the vicinity of a burrow were monitored with small microphones positioned inside and outside the cavity. For 150 presentations of calls of 15 individuals in 12 burrows, 134 were amplified and 16 were attenuated (range –6–13 dB). The fundamental resonant frequency of burrows, measured with broadcast noise and pure tones, averaged 814 Hz (range 302–1361 Hz) and covaried with burrow length. The dominant frequency of the calls of burrow occupants (average 1062 Hz, range 636–1459 Hz) was not correlated with the fundamental resonant frequency of these cavities. In burrows with low resonant frequencies, externally broadcast calls with high dominant frequencies were attenuated, or amplified to a lower extent than calls with lower dominant frequencies. The dominant frequencies of the calls experienced shifts towards the burrows’ fundamental resonant frequencies. The amplification of calls inside burrows of E. emiliopugini exhibits manifest variability, with considerable potential for facilitating acoustic interactions in this species. Received: 18 July 1999 / Received in revised form: 19 July 1999 / Accepted: 25 July 1999 相似文献
17.
J.C. Mitani W.J. Sanders J.S. Lwanga T.L. Windfelder 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2001,49(2-3):187-195
Evaluating the nature and significance of predation on populations of wild primates has been difficult given a paucity of
data regarding the phenomenon. Here we addressed this problem in a 37-month study of the predatory behavior of crowned hawk-eagles
living at the Ngogo study site in Kibale National Park, Uganda. We collected prey remains underneath the nests of two pairs
of eagles and census data on potential prey species to investigate prey selection and the ecological impact of predation on
the Ngogo primate population. Results indicate that primates form the vast majority of all prey items. Eagles prey selectively
on monkeys according to sex and species. Male primates were taken more often than females, while two species, redtail monkeys
and mangabeys, were captured significantly more and less, respectively, than chance expectation. In addition, there was no
bias in the age of prey: adult and non-adults were killed in numbers roughly equal to their proportional representations in
the forest. Further analyses indicate that a non-trivial fraction of the entire primate population at Ngogo succumbs to crowned
hawk-eagle predation each year. These results reveal both parallels and contrasts with those reported previously. Some of
the parallels are due to similarities in prey availability, while contrasts are likely related to methodological differences
between studies, inter- individual variations in predator hunting styles, and differences in prey abundance, demography, and
behavior.
Received: 29 March 2000 / Revised: 6 June 2000 / Accepted: 15 October 2000 相似文献
18.
Scott Nunes Peter A. Zugger Anne L. Engh Kurt O. Reinhart Kay E. Holekamp 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,40(3):199-207
We examined the effects of food provisioning on the natal dispersal behavior of Belding's ground squirrels (Spermophilus beldingi). We provided extra food to adult and yearling females in their maternal territories during pregnancy and lactation, and
to offspring of these females in their natal areas for 6 weeks after weaning. We used unprovisioned young of unprovisioned
mothers as controls. Provisioning influenced the probability of dispersal from the natal area by female but not male S. beldingi. All surviving male S.␣beldingi dispersed by 55 weeks of age, regardless of whether they and their mothers received extra food. By contrast, we observed
a significant trend, beginning 3 weeks after weaning and continuing through the yearling year, for a greater proportion of
provisioned than control female S. beldingi to emigrate from the natal area. Competition for food did not appear to influence natal dispersal of females. However, overall
population density, density of females weaning litters, and rates of aggression and vigilance among these females, were higher
in provisioned than control areas, suggesting that competition for non-food resources was unusually intense in provisioned
areas. We propose that juvenile female, but not juvenile male, S. beldingi may emigrate from the natal site to increase access to areas with low densities of conspecifics. Together with findings of
earlier workers, our results suggest that spatial and temporal distributions of environmental resources are important influences
on the dispersal behavior of female ground squirrels.
Received: 28 February 1996 / Accepted after revision: 16 October 1996 相似文献
19.
Ideal free distributions under predation risk 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
A. L. Moody Alasdair I. Houston John M. McNamara 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1996,38(2):131-143
We examine the trade-off between gathering food and avoiding predation in the context of patch use by a group of animals.
Often a forager will have to choose between feeding sites that differ in both energetic gain rate and predation risk. The
ideal site will have a high gain rate and low risk of predation. However, intake rate will often decrease when the patch is
shared with other foragers and it may be optimal for some individuals to feed elsewhere. Within the framework of ideal free
theory, we investigate the distribution of foragers that will equalise individual fitness gains. We focus on a two-patch environment
with continuous inputs of food. With reference to existing experimental studies, we examine the effects of risk dilution,
food input rates and an animal’s expectations of the future. We identify the effect of total animal numbers when one patch
is subject to predation risk and the other is safe. Conditions under which the difference in intake rate in the two patches
is constant are identified, as are conditions in which the ratio of animals in the two patches is constant. If current conditions
do not alter future expectations an increase in input rates to the patches promotes increased use of the risky patch. Yet,
if conditions are assumed to persist indefinitely the opposite effect is seen. When both patches are subject to predation
risk, dilution of risk favours more extreme distributions, and may lead to more than one stable distribution. The results
of these models are used to critically analyse previous work on the energetic equivalence of risk. This paper is intended
to help guide the development of new experimental studies into the energy-risk trade-off.
Received: 10 February 1995/Accepted after revision: 1 October 1995 相似文献
20.
On the evolutionary stability of female infanticide 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
Territoriality among female rodents may have evolved as an adaptation to intraspecific competition for resources or, alternatively,
to defend pups against infanticide. In order to evaluate the latter, we analyse the conditions that allow an infanticidal
strategy to invade a population of non-infanticidal females, and the circumstances under which infanticide may become an evolutionarily
stable strategy (ESS). Our game theoretical analyses indicate that infanticide has to be associated with some direct (cannibalism)
or indirect (reduced competition) resource benefits in order to invade a non-infanticidal population. We also expect that
females will primarily kill litters of nearby neighbors, thereby removing the closest competitors while keeping costs at a
low level. However, once established in a population, infanticide may be an ESS, even if females do not gain any resource
benefits. This is theoretically possible if a female through infanticide can reduce the possibility that other, potentially
infanticidal, females establish and/or stay close to her nest. While behavioral data indicate that these special circumstances
sometimes occur, they may be too specific to apply generally to small rodents. Therefore, we expect that the evolutionary
stability of infanticide often requires resource benefits, and that female infanticide in small rodents may, in fact, be a
consequence rather than a cause of territoriality.
Received: 27 June 1996 / Accepted after revision: 28 December 1996 相似文献