共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Uwe Greggers Caspar Schöning Jacqueline Degen Randolf Menzel 《Die Naturwissenschaften》2013,100(8):805-809
Harmonic radar tracking was used to record the flights of scout bees during takeoff and initial flight path of two honeybee swarms. One swarm remained intact and performed a full flight to a destination beyond the range of the harmonic radar, while a second swarm disintegrated within the range of the radar and most of the bees returned to the queen. The initial stretch of the full flight is characterized by accelerating speed, whereas the disintegrating swarm flew steadily at low speed. The two scouts in the swarm displaying full flight performed characteristic flight maneuvers. They flew at high speed when traveling in the direction of their destination and slowed down or returned over short stretches at low speed. Scouts in the disintegrating swarm did not exhibit the same kind of characteristic flight performance. Our data support the streaker bee hypothesis proposing that scout bees guide the swarm by traveling at high speed in the direction of the new nest site for short stretches of flight and slowing down when reversing flight direction. 相似文献
2.
3.
4.
Randolf Menzel 《Die Naturwissenschaften》1983,70(10):504-511
Conditioning experiments with bees demonstrate surprising similarities between the well studied laboratory mammals and honeybees. These similarities appear both on the behavioral level and the neural organization of memory. The paper speculates on the basis of these similarities, and the idea of common neural elements is favored. 相似文献
5.
Honeybees, Apis spp., maintain elevated temperatures inside their nests to accelerate brood development and to facilitate defense against predators.
We present an additional defensive function of elevating nest temperature: honeybees generate a brood-comb fever in response
to colonial infection by the heat-sensitive pathogen Ascosphaera apis. This response occurs before larvae are killed, suggesting that either honeybee workers detect the infection before symptoms
are visible, or that larvae communicate the ingestion of the pathogen. This response is a striking example of convergent evolution
between this "superorganism" and other fever-producing animals.
Received: 2 September 1999 / Accepted in revised form: 28 February 2000 相似文献
6.
7.
Michael L. Smith Madeleine M. Ostwald J. Carter Loftus Thomas D. Seeley 《Die Naturwissenschaften》2014,101(10):783-790
Social insect colonies, like individual organisms, must decide as they develop how to allocate optimally their resources among survival, growth, and reproduction. Only when colonies reach a certain state do they switch from investing purely in survival and growth to investing also in reproduction. But how do worker bees within a colony detect that their colony has reached the state where it is adaptive to begin investing in reproduction? Previous work has shown that larger honeybee colonies invest more in reproduction (i.e., the production of drones and queens), however, the term ‘larger’ encompasses multiple colony parameters including number of adult workers, size of the nest, amount of brood, and size of the honey stores. These colony parameters were independently increased in this study to test which one(s) would increase a colony’s investment in reproduction via males. This was assayed by measuring the construction of drone comb, the special type of comb in which drones are reared. Only an increase in the number of workers stimulated construction of drone comb. Colonies with over 4,000 workers began building drone comb, independent of the other colony parameters. These results show that attaining a critical number of workers is the key parameter for honeybee colonies to start to shift resources towards reproduction. These findings are relevant to other social systems in which a group’s members must adjust their behavior as a function of the group’s size. 相似文献
8.
H. Randall Hepburn Sarah E. Radloff Orawan Duangphakdee Mananya Phaincharoen 《Die Naturwissenschaften》2009,96(6):719-723
Beeswaxes of honeybee species share some homologous neutral lipids; but species-specific differences remain. We analysed behavioural
variation for wax choice in honeybees, calculated the Euclidean distances for different beeswaxes and assessed the relationship
of Euclidean distances to wax choice. We tested the beeswaxes of Apis mellifera capensis, Apis florea, Apis cerana and Apis dorsata and the plant and mineral waxes Japan, candelilla, bayberry and ozokerite as sheets placed in colonies of A. m. capensis, A. florea and A. cerana. A. m. capensis accepted the four beeswaxes but removed Japan and bayberry wax and ignored candelilla and ozokerite. A. cerana colonies accepted the wax of A. cerana, A. florea and A. dorsata but rejected or ignored that of A. m. capensis, the plant and mineral waxes. A. florea colonies accepted A. cerana, A. dorsata and A. florea wax but rejected that of A. m. capensis. The Euclidean distances for the beeswaxes are consistent with currently prevailing phylogenies for Apis. Despite post-speciation chemical differences in the beeswaxes, they remain largely acceptable interspecifically while the
plant and mineral waxes are not chemically close enough to beeswax for their acceptance. 相似文献
9.
10.
The nests of European honeybees (Apis mellifera) are organised into wax combs that contain many cells with a hexagonal structure. Many previous studies on comb-building behaviour have been made in order to understand how bees produce this geometrical structure; however, it still remains a mystery. Direct construction of hexagons by bees was suggested previously, while a recent hypothesis postulated the self-organised construction of hexagonal comb cell arrays; however, infrared and thermographic video observations of comb building in the present study failed to support the self-organisation hypothesis because bees were shown to be engaged in direct construction. Bees used their antennae, mandibles and legs in a regular sequence to manipulate the wax, while some bees supported their work by actively warming the wax. During the construction of hexagonal cells, the wax temperature was between 33.6 and 37.6 °C. This is well below 40 °C, i.e. the temperature at which wax is assumed to exist in the liquid equilibrium that is essential for self-organised building. 相似文献
11.
Giurfa M 《Die Naturwissenschaften》2004,91(5):228-231
We studied the influence of the conditioning procedure on color discrimination by free-flying honeybees. We asked whether absolute and differential conditioning result in different discrimination capabilities for the same pairs of colored targets. In absolute conditioning, bees were rewarded on a single color; in differential conditioning, bees were rewarded on the same color but an alternative, non-rewarding, similar color was also visible. In both conditioning procedures, bees learned their respective task and could also discriminate the training stimulus from a novel stimulus that was perceptually different from the trained one. Discrimination between perceptually closer stimuli was possible after differential conditioning but not after absolute conditioning. Differences in attention inculcated by these training procedures may underlie the different discrimination performances of the bees. 相似文献
12.
We report the results of a simple experiment to determine whether honeybees feed their small hive beetle nest parasites. Honeybees incarcerate the beetles in cells constructed of plant resins and continually guard them. The longevity of incarcerated beetles greatly exceeds their metabolic reserves. We show that survival of small hive beetles derives from behavioural mimicry by which the beetles induce the bees to feed them trophallactically. Electronic supplementary material to this paper can be obtained by using the Springer LINK server located at htpp://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00114-002-0326-y. 相似文献
13.
Bordoni Adele Matejkova Zuzana Chimenti Lorenzo Massai Lorenzo Perito Brunella Dapporto Leonardo Turillazzi Stefano 《Die Naturwissenschaften》2019,106(11-12):1-7
The Science of Nature - Orchids attach their pollinaria (cohesive masses of pollen) to specific body parts of flower visitors, but usually not to the hairy and scaly body parts of flower-visiting... 相似文献
14.
Orientation and navigation during adult transport between nests in the ant Cataglypis iberica 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Cataglyphis iberica is a polydomous ant species in which adult transports between nests are frequently observed. When pairs of workers were captured
and released at the same location, the transporters (Ts) fled directly towards their destination nest and reached it in most
of the cases. The transportees (Te), on the other hand, fled in the opposite direction and only a third of them eventually
reached their nest of departure. Additional experiments suggest that this result may be explained by the fact that the Ts
ants have a memory of the compass direction of the nest they are heading to and that they adjust their course by using a sequence
of memorised landmarks. As regards to the Te, the reversal of their direction of transport seems to be based essentially on
celestial cues.
Received: 20 October 1999 / Accepted in revised form: 10 May 2000 相似文献
15.
16.
Randolf Menzel Jacqueline Fuchs Leonard Nadler Benjamin Weiss Nicole Kumbischinski Daniel Adebiyi Sergej Hartfil Uwe Greggers 《Die Naturwissenschaften》2010,97(8):763-767
Honeybees use their visual flow field to measure flight distance. It has been suggested that the experience of serial landmarks
encountered on the flight toward a feeding place contributes to distance estimation. Here, we address this question by tracing
the flight paths of individual bees with a harmonic radar system. Bees were trained along an array of three landmarks (tents),
and the distance between these landmarks was either increased or decreased under two test conditions. We find that absolute
distance estimation dominates the search for the feeding place, but serial position effects are also found. In the latter
case, bees search only or additionally at locations determined by serial experience of the landmarks. 相似文献
17.
18.
19.