共查询到5条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
The ecological constraints model predicts that daily travel distance and home range size of social animals will increase as group size increases in order to meet the dietary needs of additional group members. This theory has been supported more predominantly by studies of frugivorous primate species than by studies of folivorous species. We examined the ranging patterns of mountain gorillas in Bwindi Impenetrable National Park, Uganda, who include both herbaceous vegetation and fruit in their diet, to determine how ecological, behavioral, and social parameters influence movement patterns. Data were collected from three groups of gorillas with overlapping home ranges at a low-altitude location (1,450–1,800 m) and one group at a high-altitude location (2,100–2,500 m) in Bwindi from September 2001 to August 2002. We analyzed daily travel distance and home range size in relation to group size, while also considering patterns of frugivory, rainfall, and location (proxy for food availability) within the park. Both daily travel distance and home range size were positively related to group size. In addition, the degree of frugivory positively influenced daily travel distance and home range size, while rainfall negatively influenced daily travel distance only. Finally, groups at the low-altitude location, with higher fruit availability, traveled less than the group at the high-altitude location. These results demonstrate that mountain gorillas in Bwindi provide support for the ecological constraints model, but further studies are needed to determine how fine-scale spatial and temporal availability of food resources influence movement patterns. Ranging patterns of Bwindi gorillas are compared to those observed in other gorilla populations in the context of the ecological constraints model.Communicated by D. Watts 相似文献
2.
J.C. Mitani W.J. Sanders J.S. Lwanga T.L. Windfelder 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2001,49(2-3):187-195
Evaluating the nature and significance of predation on populations of wild primates has been difficult given a paucity of
data regarding the phenomenon. Here we addressed this problem in a 37-month study of the predatory behavior of crowned hawk-eagles
living at the Ngogo study site in Kibale National Park, Uganda. We collected prey remains underneath the nests of two pairs
of eagles and census data on potential prey species to investigate prey selection and the ecological impact of predation on
the Ngogo primate population. Results indicate that primates form the vast majority of all prey items. Eagles prey selectively
on monkeys according to sex and species. Male primates were taken more often than females, while two species, redtail monkeys
and mangabeys, were captured significantly more and less, respectively, than chance expectation. In addition, there was no
bias in the age of prey: adult and non-adults were killed in numbers roughly equal to their proportional representations in
the forest. Further analyses indicate that a non-trivial fraction of the entire primate population at Ngogo succumbs to crowned
hawk-eagle predation each year. These results reveal both parallels and contrasts with those reported previously. Some of
the parallels are due to similarities in prey availability, while contrasts are likely related to methodological differences
between studies, inter- individual variations in predator hunting styles, and differences in prey abundance, demography, and
behavior.
Received: 29 March 2000 / Revised: 6 June 2000 / Accepted: 15 October 2000 相似文献
3.
Data on langur (Presbytis entellus) populations were gathered from the literature to test the importance of three selective pressures in determining group size
and composition: predation pressure, intergroup resource defense, and conspecific threat. There were no detectable difference
in the size of groups in populations facing nearly intact predator communities compared to those populations where predators
were severely reduced in number or absent, although there was a trend for the number of adult males per group to increase
in areas with nearly intact predator communities. Using population density as an indirect measure of the frequency of intrusions
into a group’s home range and thereby as an index of the demographic pressure favoring resource defense, we predicted that
higher densities would result in larger defensive coalitions and higher numbers of females per group. This prediction was
not upheld. Our third selective pressure, conspecific threat, encompasses those selective forces resulting from physical attack
on females, infants, and juveniles. Our index of conspecific threat uses the number of non-group males divided by the number
of bisexual groups, because in langurs, the major source of conspecific threat derives from non-group males who, following
group take-over, kill infants, wound females, and expel juveniles from groups. This index of conspecific threat was strongly
related to the mean number of resident females, was weakly related to the mean group size, but was not related to the number
of males in the group. In addition, as predicted, populations with a high index of conspecific threat had higher levels of
juvenile expulsion. These analyses were corroborated by a simulation model which used a computer-generated series of null
populations to calculate expected slopes of immatures regressed on adult females. These randomly generated populations, matched
to means and ranges of real populations, allowed us to determine if deviations of the observed slopes from the expected null
slopes could be explained by variation in predation pressure, population density, or conspecific threat. We found no evidence
that predation pressure was associated with decreases in immature survival in smaller groups, as would be predicted by the
predation-avoidance hypothesis. We found no evidence that immature survival was compromised by small group size in high-density
populations, as would be predicted by the resource-defense hypothesis. However, as the index of conspecific threat increased,
groups with larger numbers of females were more successful than groups with fewer females in reducing mortality or expulsion
of immatures. Overall, conspecific threat received the strongest support as a selective pressure influencing langur group
size and composition, suggesting that this selective pressure should be evaluated more widely as a factor influencing composition
of animal groups.
Received: 23 January 1995/Accepted after revision: 18 February 1996 相似文献
4.
K. E. Arnold 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2000,48(5):385-391
Living with relatives can be beneficial to individuals via the evolution of kin-directed altruism, but this is tempered by
the increased risk of inbreeding. Therefore, in social species, the ability to recognise relatives can be highly advantageous.
This study focuses on kin discrimination in the Lake Eacham rainbowfish, Melanotaenia eachamensis, an endangered freshwater species from north-east Queensland, Australia. First, I examined kin recognition abilities when
a combination of both chemical and visual recognition cues was available. When given a choice of shoaling with same-sex groups,
females spent significantly longer with full-sibs rather than half-sibs, full-sibs rather than non-relatives and half-sibs
rather than non-relatives. Males spent significantly longer shoaling with full-brothers versus half-brothers, but showed no
other shoalmate preferences. Second, in the presence of only chemical cues, females did not discriminate among groups of different
levels of relatedness, but males showed a non-significant tendency to associate with full-sibs rather than non-relatives.
Male shoaling behaviour seemed to be more influenced by factors other than relatedness, e.g. intra-sexual competition. Finally,
I found that the shoaling preferences of females changed when exposed to groups of males. Females preferred to associate with
non-relatives rather than half-brothers and non-relatives rather than full-brothers. There was no significant difference in
the time spent with half-brothers versus full-brothers. Taken together, my results suggest that females have very good kin
recognition abilities. They prefer to shoal with female relatives but avoid male relatives, and so are able to balance the
benefits of nepotism and the costs of incest.
Received: 2 May 2000 / Revised: 26 July 2000 / Accepted: 30 July 2000 相似文献
5.
Antipredator behavior of a social desert rodent: footdrumming and alarm calling in the great gerbil, Rhombomys opiums 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
J. A. Randall Konstantin A. Rogovin Debra M. Shier 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2000,48(2):110-118
We sought to understand why a social, desert rodent, the great gerbil, Rhombomys opimus, expends energy and possible risk of predation by footdrumming and vocalizing in the presence of a diversity of terrestrial
predators: snakes, monitor lizards, polecats, foxes, and humans. Behavioral observations, human approaches, and experiments
with tethered predators revealed that both male and female gerbils called and footdrummed in the presence of offspring, close
relatives, and potential mates. Because adults called more often when pups were present, and solitary gerbils seldom gave
an alarm, the alarm behavior probably warns conspecifics, especially vulnerable offspring, of potential danger. We also found
that gerbils altered alarm behavior with the type of predator. They drummed more in the burrow when a dog that could not enter
the burrow was present, and they drummed more out of the burrow in response to a snake that could enter the burrow. Gerbils
vocalized and stood in an alert posture in response to all stimuli. The different footdrumming responses of gerbils to terrestrial
predators seems related to the hunting style and type of risk posed by the predator, especially its ability to enter the burrow
system.
Received: 23 August 1999 / Received in revised form: 6 December 1999 / Accepted: 25 February 2000 相似文献