共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Individual fish commonly leave the relative safety of the shoal to approach potential predators at a distance. Not all members of a shoal are equally likely to initiate such predator inspection visits. Here, we show for the first time that the current hunger state of individual fish strongly influences their predator inspection behaviour, as well as their foraging rate, in the face of predation hazard. When all members of threespine stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus) test shoals were in a similar hunger state, they were equally likely to inspect a trout predator model alone and did not differ in the frequency of their inspection visits or foraging rate. However, when individual sticklebacks in a shoal differed in their hunger state, the food-deprived (i.e. hungrier) member of the shoal fed at a higher rate, was significantly more likely to initiate solitary predator inspection visits, and inspected the predator model significantly more often than its less hungry (i.e. well-fed) shoal mates. Individual fish which inspected the predator model more frequently also tended to have higher feeding rates. The results indicate that the hungrier fish in a shoal are more willing to take greater risks to inspect a potential threat at a distance, compared with their well-fed shoal mates, and suggest that they may gain a foraging benefit in doing so. If marked asymmetries in hunger state exist among members of fish shoals, then mutual cooperation during predator inspection visits may be difficult to achieve because well-fed individuals are not as likely to initiate or participate in inspection visits as are hungry individuals.Correspondence to: J.-G.J. Godin 相似文献
2.
Grant E. Brown Alix C. Rive Maud C. O. Ferrari Douglas P. Chivers 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2006,61(1):9-16
Prey animals often have to face a dynamic tradeoff between the costs of antipredator behavior and the benefits of other fitness-related activities such as foraging and reproduction. According to the threat-sensitive predator avoidance hypothesis, prey animals should match the intensity of their antipredator behavior to the degree of immediate threat posed by the predator. Moreover, longer-term temporal variability in predation risk (over days to weeks) can shape the intensity of antipredator behavior. According to the risk allocation hypothesis, changing the background level of risk for several days is often enough to change the response intensity of the prey to a given stimulus. As the background level of risk increases, the response intensity of the prey decreases. In this study, we tested for possible interactions between immediate threat-sensitive responses to varying levels of current perceived risk and temporal variability in background risk experienced over the past 3 days. Juvenile convict cichlids were preexposed to either low or high frequencies of predation risk (using conspecific chemical alarm cues) for 3 days and were then tested for a response to one of five concentrations (100, 50, 25, 12.5%, or a distilled water control). According to the threat-sensitive predator avoidance hypothesis, we found greater intensity responses to greater concentrations of alarm cues. Moreover, in accordance with the risk allocation hypothesis, we found that cichlids previously exposed to the high background level of risk exhibited a lower overall intensity response to each alarm cue concentration than those exposed to the low background level of risk. It is interesting to note that we found that the background level of risk over the past 3 days influenced the threshold level of response to varying concentrations of alarm cues. Indeed, the minimum stimulus concentration that evoked a behavioral response was lower for fish exposed to high background levels of predation than those exposed to low background levels of predation. These results illustrate a remarkable interplay between immediate (current) risk and background risk in shaping the intensity of antipredator responses. 相似文献
3.
Clare D. FitzGibbon 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1994,34(2):139-148
Summary When Thomson's gazelles (Gazella thomsoni) detect stalking predators, such as cheetahs (Acinonyx jubatus) and lions (Panthera leo), they often approach and follow the predator for up to 72 min (average 14 min). Coursing predators are rarely approached. Gazelle groups were more likely to approach cheetahs if the groups were larger, if the vegetation was low, or if the cheetahs came closer to the group. Immature gazelles were more likely to approach than adults, and a higher proportion of group members participated in inspection behaviour in small groups than in large ones. Gazelles approached closer in less risky situations: if they were in larger groups or if the vegetation was low. Inspection behaviour caused cheetahs to move further between rests and between hunting attempts. Approaching cheetahs was risky, particularly for younger gazelles (probability of being killed while inspecting a cheetah was 1 in 5000 approaches for adults and 1 in 417 approaches for half-grown/adolescent gazelles), and the risks were higher than monitoring cheetahs from a distance. The time costs of predator inspection were also considerable (less than 4.2% of daylight time budget), suggesting that the benefits must be substantial to offset these costs. The results suggested that inspection behaviour was multifunctional, causing stalking predators to move out of the vicinity, enabling gazelles to monitor the predators' movements, and providing an opportunity, particularly for younger animals, to learn about predators. By approaching, gazelles also inform predators that they have been detected and alert other gazelles to the predators' presence. 相似文献
4.
Jennifer L. Verdolin 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2006,60(4):457-464
Although there is ample evidence for the generality of foraging and predation trade-offs in aquatic systems, its application to terrestrial systems is less comprehensive. In this review, meta-analysis was used to analyze experiments on giving-up-densities in terrestrial systems to evaluate the overall magnitude of predation risk on foraging behavior and experimental conditions mediating its effect. Results indicate a large and significant decrease in foraging effort as a consequence of increased predation risk. Whether experiments were conducted under natural or artificial conditions produced no change in the overall effect predation had on foraging. Odor and live predators as a correlate of predation risk had weaker and nonsignificant effects compared to habitat characteristics. The meta-analysis suggests that the effect of predation risk on foraging behavior in terrestrial systems is strongly dependent on the type of predation risk being utilized. 相似文献
5.
Ideal free distributions under predation risk 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
A. L. Moody Alasdair I. Houston John M. McNamara 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1996,38(2):131-143
We examine the trade-off between gathering food and avoiding predation in the context of patch use by a group of animals.
Often a forager will have to choose between feeding sites that differ in both energetic gain rate and predation risk. The
ideal site will have a high gain rate and low risk of predation. However, intake rate will often decrease when the patch is
shared with other foragers and it may be optimal for some individuals to feed elsewhere. Within the framework of ideal free
theory, we investigate the distribution of foragers that will equalise individual fitness gains. We focus on a two-patch environment
with continuous inputs of food. With reference to existing experimental studies, we examine the effects of risk dilution,
food input rates and an animal’s expectations of the future. We identify the effect of total animal numbers when one patch
is subject to predation risk and the other is safe. Conditions under which the difference in intake rate in the two patches
is constant are identified, as are conditions in which the ratio of animals in the two patches is constant. If current conditions
do not alter future expectations an increase in input rates to the patches promotes increased use of the risky patch. Yet,
if conditions are assumed to persist indefinitely the opposite effect is seen. When both patches are subject to predation
risk, dilution of risk favours more extreme distributions, and may lead to more than one stable distribution. The results
of these models are used to critically analyse previous work on the energetic equivalence of risk. This paper is intended
to help guide the development of new experimental studies into the energy-risk trade-off.
Received: 10 February 1995/Accepted after revision: 1 October 1995 相似文献
6.
Guy Cowlishaw 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1999,45(1):67-77
This paper investigates the determinants of individual spacing behaviour in a desert baboon population (Papio cynocephalus ursinus). Patterns of neighbour proximity and neighbour density were examined among adults in four groups under different ecological and social conditions (through instantaneous sampling during focal follows). Initial analysis of these data shows that (1) the use of vertical substrates (refuges such as tall trees and cliff faces) can confound patterns of spacing, and (2) individual differences in spacing can depend on the spatial scale over which it is measured. To minimise these substrate and scale effects, this analysis focuses on animals which are off refuges and examines spacing behaviour through its underlying statistical `dimensions' (identified through factor analysis). Analysis of these dimensions indicates that sex, group size, activity-habitat and female reproductive state can all have independent effects on spacing: (1) males are more dispersed than females in smaller groups, (2) male and female dispersion increases with time spent in foraging habitats, and (3) female dispersion is reduced during lactation. According to the hypotheses tested, these results indicate that feeding competition only affects spacing behaviour during foraging while predation risk plays little or no role in spacing. Most aspects of spacing behaviour are best explained by male reproductive strategies and their social repercussions. Received: 25 May 1998 / Accepted after revision: 18 July 1998 相似文献
7.
Michael E. Fraker 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2008,62(7):1201-1205
The activity level of prey reflects a trade-off between predation risk and foraging gain. A number of theoretical and empirical
studies have shown that a prey's energetic state or the level of its resource should influence this trade-off (i.e., what
the optimal activity level at a level of predation risk is). Here, I show that the energetic state of prey may also influence
the duration of their antipredator behavioral response. Green frog tadpoles (Rana clamitans) reduced their activity level for a shorter time during exposure to the chemical cue of predatory larval dragonflies (Anax spp.) as their time since last feeding increased (i.e., as their energetic state decreased). Interestingly, the tadpoles
strongly reduced their activity level upon cue exposure in all treatments. Thus, the relative activity level of tadpoles at
different energetic states varied over time. 相似文献
8.
It has been argued that the body mass levels achieved by birds are determined by the trade-off between risks of starvation
and predation. Birds have also been found to reduce body mass in response to an increased predation risk. During migration,
the need of extra fuel for flights is obvious and crucial. In this study, migratory blackcaps (Sylvia atricapilla) were subject to an experimental stopover situation where the predation risk was manipulated by exposure to a stuffed predator.
Blackcaps that perceived an imminent risk of predation increased their food intake and fuel deposition rate during the first
period of stopover compared with a control group. The pattern of night activity indicates that birds that were exposed to
the predator also chose to leave earlier than birds in the control group. Since there was no cover present at the stopover
site, birds might have perceived the risk of predation as higher regardless of whether they were foraging or not. Under such
circumstances it has been predicted that birds should increase their foraging activity. The findings in this study clearly
indicate that birds are able to adjust their stopover behaviour to perceived predation risk.
Received: 8 January 1997 / Accepted after revision: 11 April 1997 相似文献
9.
Rodrigo A. Vásquez 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1994,34(5):375-381
It is well known that the risk of predation affects prey decision making. However, few studies have been concerned with the cues used by prey to assess this risk. Prey animals may use indirect environmental cues to assess predation hazard since direct evaluation may be dangerous. I studied the assessment of predation risk, manipulated via environmental illumination level, and the trade-off between foraging and predation hazard avoidance in the nocturnal rodentPhyllotis darwini (Rodentia: Cricetidae). In experimental arenas I simulated dark and full moon nights (which in nature correlate with low and high predation risk, respectively) and measured the immediate responses of animals to flyovers of a raptor model. Second, varying illumination only, I evaluated patch use, food consumption, central place foraging, and nocturnal variation of body weight. During flyover experiments, animals showed significantly more evasive reactions under full moon illumination than in moonless conditions. In the patch use experiments, rodents significantly increased their giving-up density and decreased their total food consumption under moonlight. On dark nights, rodents normally fed in the food patch, but when illumination was high they became central place foragers in large proportion. Moreover, the body weight of individuals decreased proportionately more during bright nights. These results strongly suggest thatP. darwini uses the level of environmental illumination as a cue to the risk of being preyed upon and may sacrifice part of its energy return to avoid risky situations. 相似文献
10.
Summary. Predation is a pervasive selective agent highly variable in space and time. Due to the costs associated with antipredator
responses, prey would be at a selective advantage if they respond to predation threats with an intensitfy matching the threat
posed by the predator. Many aquatic organisms have been shown to use chemical alarm cues present in the water to assess the
level of risk in their environment. This includes mosquito larvae which show antipredator responses to conspecific alarm cues.
In this study, we investigated the nature of the responses of larval mosquitoes Culex restuans to those cues. In our initial observations, we showed pond/population differences in the response intensity of C. restuans to alarm cues. In experiment 1, we showed that the response intensity to alarm cues could be increased by increasing the
background level of risk in the mosquitoes’ environment (by adding salamander predators) and once turned on, the response
intensity to alarm cues was likely maintained for the remainder of the mosquitoes’ aquatic life. In experiments 2 and 3, we
investigated if the increase in response intensity to alarm cues was directly correlated with the level of background risk
in the mosquitoes’ environment. When given increasing levels of background risk, mosquito larvae subsequently showed a graded
response to conspecific alarm cues. This series of experiments demonstrates that the response intensity of larval mosquitoes
to a standard concentration of alarm cues is not fixed, but rather dependent on the background level of risk in the environment.
An understanding of the background level of risk is particularly important for comparing antipredator responses of prey between
habitats. 相似文献
11.
Timothy C. Sparkes 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1996,39(6):411-417
In field surveys, laboratory observations and field-based assays of behavior, I examined the effects of size-dependent predation
risk on the interaction between size at reproductive maturity and maternal care behavior in the stream-dwelling isopod, Lirceus fontinalis. L. fontinalis exhibit population-specific sizes at reproductive maturity which result in population differences in predation risk during
the adult phase. Females from streams containing salamander larvae (that prefer small prey) mature at large sizes and then
become relatively safe from predation. Females from streams containing fish (that consume all size classes of prey equally)
mature at small sizes and remain at risk. I tested whether these differences in expected survival were reflected in the behavior
of females during the maternal phase (i.e., the period during which females exhibit maternal care). Female L. fontinalis carry developing juveniles inside a brood pouch. I simulated predatory attacks on gravid female L. fontinalis from the different population types and found that female behavior correlated with population differences in risk. When “attacked”,
females from streams with predatory fish (that experience high risk to adult females) released juveniles from the brood pouch,
whereas females from populations with predatory salamander larvae (that pose relatively little risk to adult females) did
not release juveniles. I discuss the results with reference to the joint evolution of behavioral and life history traits.
Received: 6 March 1996 /Accepted after revision: 12 August 1996 相似文献
12.
Adrian Vallin Sven Jakobsson Christer Wiklund 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2007,61(9):1419-1424
Large eyespots on the wings of butterflies and moths have been ascribed generally intimidating qualities by creating a frightening
image of a bird or mammal much larger than the insect bearing the eyespots. However, evidence for this anti-predator adaptation
has been largely anecdotal and only recently were peacock butterflies, Inachis io, shown to effectively thwart attacks from blue tits, Parus caeruleus. Here, we test whether large eyespots on lepidopterans are generally effective in preventing attacks from small passerines
and whether the size of insect or bird can influence the outcome of interactions. We staged experiments between the larger
eyed hawkmoths, Smerinthus ocellatus, and the smaller peacock butterflies, I. io, and the larger great tits, Parus major, and the smaller blue tits, P. caeruleus. Survival differed substantially between the insect species with 21 of 24 peacock butterflies, but only 6 of 27 eyed hawkmoths,
surviving attacks from the birds. Thus, surprisingly, the smaller prey survived to a higher extent, suggesting that factors
other than insect size may be important. However, great tits were less easily intimidated by the insects’ eyespots and deimatic
behaviour and consumed 16 of 26, but the blue tits only 8 of 25, of the butterflies and hawkmoths. Our results demonstrate
that eyespots per se do not guarantee survival and that these two insects bearing equally large eyespots are not equally well
protected against predation. 相似文献
13.
Frank Götmark 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,40(1):41-49
Conspicuous color patterns in birds may lead to increased risk of predation. Alternatively, bright birds may be aposematic
or unprofitable prey, which leads to decreased predation. During four autumns, I examined whether the black-and-white plumage
of (stuffed) magpies Pica pica increases or reduces the risk of attack by migrating goshawks Accipiter gentilis. Attack risk was higher for normal magpies than for cryptic, brown magpies whether the mounts were placed near one another
or far apart. However, the brown magpie may have been avoided because of its novelty. For magpies and cryptic jays Garrulus glandarius exposed together, the attack risk was similar. In 2 years, magpie and jay mounts were also exposed far apart. In 1994, with
an invasion of migrating jays, attack risk was much higher for jays than for magpies. In 1995, with a normal density of jays,
hawks attacked the magpies more often. The results demonstrate frequency-dependent prey selection by goshawks, which would
influence any predation cost of bright plumage. The attacks on normally colored magpie mounts suggest that magpies are not
aposematic. Trials with photographs and human observers indicated that normal magpies were somewhat easier to detect than
jays. The plumage of the magpie possibly increases the risk of predation, but may be favoured by sexual or social selection.
Received: 18 January 1996 / Accepted after revision: 6 October 1996 相似文献
14.
Making sense of predator scents: investigating the sophistication of predator assessment abilities of fathead minnows 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
According to the threat-sensitive predator avoidance hypothesis, selection favors prey that accurately assess the degree of threat posed by a predator and adjust their anti-predator response to match the level of risk. Many species of animals rely on chemical cues to estimate predation risk; however, the information content conveyed in these chemical signatures is not well understood. We tested the threat-sensitive predator avoidance hypothesis by determining the specificity of the information conveyed to prey in the chemical signature of their predator. We found that fathead minnows (Pimephales promelas) could determine the degree of threat posed by northern pike (Esox lucius) based on the concentration of chemical cues used. The proportion of minnows that exhibited an anti-predator response when exposed to a predator cue increased as the concentration of the pike cue used increased. More surprisingly, the prey could also distinguish large pike from small pike based on their odor alone. The minnows responded more intensely to cues of small pike than to cues of large pike. In this predator–prey system small pike likely represent a greater threat than large pike.Communicated by A. Mathis 相似文献
15.
Behavioral resource depression and decaying perceived risk of predation in two species of coexisting gerbils 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
Burt P. Kotler 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1992,30(3-4):239-244
Summary Behavioral resource depression occurs when the behavior of prey individuals changes in response to the presence of a predator, resulting in a reduction of the encounter rate of the predator with its prey. Here I present experimental evidence on the response of two species of gerbils (Gerbillus allenbyi and G. pyramidum) to the presence of barn owls. I conducted the experiments in a large aviary. Both gerbils responded to the presence of barn owl predators by foraging in fewer resource patches (seed trays) and by quitting foraged resource patches at a higher resource harvest rate (giving-up density of resource; GUD). This reduced the amount of time gerbils were exposed to owl predation, and hence the encounter rate of owls with gerbils, i.e., behavioral resource depression. Thus, the presence of owls imposes a foraging cost on gerbils due to risk of predation, and also on the owls themselves due to resource depression. I then examined how resource depression relaxed over time following exposure to owls. In the days following an encounter with the predator, the reduction in foraging activity for both gerbil species eased. Increasing numbers of trays were foraged each day, and GUDs in seed trays declined. The two gerbils differed in their rate of recovery, with G. pyramidum returning to prepredator levels of foraging after 1 or 2 nights and G. allenbyi taking 5 nights or longer. Interspecific differences in recovery rates may be based on differences between the species in vulnerability to predation and/or ability to detect the presence of predators. The differences in recovery rates may be due to optimal memory windows or decay rates, where differences between species are based on risk of predation or on how perceived risk changes with time since a predator was last encountered. Finally, differences between or among competitors in recovery from resource depression may provide foraging opportunities in time for the species which recover most quickly and may have implications for species coexistence. 相似文献
16.
Axel Strauß Katrin Y. Solmsdorff Roger Pech Jens Jacob 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2008,62(10):1551-1558
Predators can strongly influence the microhabitat use and foraging behaviour of prey. In a large-scale replicated field experiment
in East Gippsland, Australia, we tested the effects of reduced alien red fox (Vulpes vulpes) and alien wild dog (Canis lupus familiaris) abundance (treatment) on native bush rat (Rattus fuscipes) behaviour. Bush rats are exposed to two main guilds of predators, namely mammalian carnivores and birds of prey. Tracking
rat movements using the spool-and-line technique revealed that, in treatment sites, rats used ground cover, which provides
shelter from predators, less often than at unmanipulated fox and wild dog abundance (non-treatment sites). In treatment sites,
rats more frequently moved on logs where they would have been exposed to hunting foxes and dogs than in non-treatment sites.
Furthermore, in treatments, rats showed a preference for understorey but not in non-treatments. Hence, bush rats adapted their
behaviour to removal of alien terrestrial predators. Giving-up densities (GUDs) indicated no treatment effects on the marginal
feeding rate of bush rats. Interestingly, GUDs were higher in open patches than in sheltered patches, suggesting higher perceived
predation risk of bush rats during foraging at low versus high cover. The lack of treatment effects on GUDs but the clear
response of bush rats to cover may be explained by the impact of predators other than foxes and wild dogs. 相似文献
17.
Jaimie T. A. Dick Robert W. Elwood W. Ian Montgomery 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1995,37(6):393-398
Previous studies have shown that differential predation by males on moulted female congenerics may be largely responsible for the elimination and replacement of the native Irish freshwater amphipod Gammarus duebeni celticus by the introduced G. pulex. Predation of moulted females occurs both shortly after their release from precopulatory mate-guarding and whilst they are being guarded by their mates. In the present study, two hypotheses concerning the underlying cause(s) of the differential predation pattern are tested. Firstly, female G. d. celticus may be more vulnerable to predation than female G. pulex due to the former being released from precopula guarding with the new exoskeleton in a less hardened state. Secondly, G. pulex may be an inherently more aggressive species than G. d. celticus during predatory interactions over guarded females. The first experiment indicated that differential predation was not mediated by species differences in the state of the female exoskeleton at the time of release from precopula by guarding males. The second experiment, however, showed that male G. pulex were significantly more aggressive than male G. d. celticus in attacking both guarding male congenerics and guarded moulted female congenerics. In addition, in defence against predatory attacks, paired male and female G. pulex were significantly more aggressive than paired male and female G. d. celticus. These differences in aggressive behaviour led to a significantly higher frequency of predation on G. d. celticus females than on G. pulex females, and also explains this finding in previous studies. It is concluded that differential predation due to differences in aggressive behaviour may explain the pattern of replacement between these species. 相似文献
18.
Predation risk affects courtship and attractiveness of competing threespine stickleback males 总被引:6,自引:3,他引:6
Ulrika Candolin 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,41(2):81-87
The effect of predation risk and male-male competition on male courtship behaviour and attractiveness to females was studied
in the threespine stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus) by presenting dummy or live females to solitary and competing males under different predation risks. In the presence of
a predator, males decreased courtship activity. Different courtship components were, however, adjusted to different extents
and in opposing directions to predation risk, probably because the single components may have varied in riskiness. The presence
of a competing male decreased overall courtship activity, but increased the frequency of zigzags, suggesting zigzagging to
be a competitive strategy against other males. In the presence of a predator male courtship activity was not affected by a
competitor. Female mate choice correlated with the males' previous frequency of zigzags towards a dummy female. However, when
a live female paid attention to a male, the male decreased zigzagging and instead increased leading and fanning behaviours,
probably trying to attract the female to the nest to mate. Predation risk affected the attractiveness of males as females
reduced their attention to a male when he faced a predator and reduced his courtship activity. As females instead increased
their attention to a competing male that had increased his courtship activity, due to decreased competition, males clearly
are balancing mating opportunities against predator avoidance. When males vary in their susceptibility to predators, predation
risk may thus affect mating success of competing males.
Received: 31 January 1997 / Accepted after revision: 15 April 1997 相似文献
19.
Animals face trade-offs between predation risk and foraging success depending on their location in the landscape; for example, individuals that remain near a common shelter may be safe from predation but incur stronger competition for resources. Despite a long tradition of theoretical exploration of the relationships among foraging success, conspecific competition, predation risk, and population distribution in a heterogeneous environment, the scenario we describe here has not been explored theoretically. We construct a model of habitat use rules to predict the distribution of a local population (prey sharing a common shelter and foraging across surrounding habitats). Our model describes realized habitat quality as a ratio of density- and location-dependent mortality to density-dependent growth. We explore how the prey distribution around a shelter is expected to change as the parameters governing the strength of density dependence, landscape characteristics, and local abundance vary. Within the range of parameters where prey spend some time away from shelter but remain site-attached, the prey density decreases away from shelter. As the distance at which prey react to predators increases, the population range generally increases. At intermediate reaction distances, however, increases in the reaction distance lead to decreases in the maximum foraging distance because of increased evenness in the population distribution. As total abundance increases, the population range increases, average population density increases, and realized quality decreases. The magnitude of these changes differs in, for example, ‘high-’ and ‘low-visibility’ landscapes where prey can detect predators at different distances. 相似文献
20.
Grant E. Brown Camille J. Macnaughton Chris K. Elvidge Indar Ramnarine Jean-Guy J. Godin 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2009,63(5):699-706
The antipredator behaviour of prey organisms is shaped by a series of threat-sensitive trade-offs between the benefits associated
with successful predator avoidance and a suite of other fitness-related behaviours such as foraging, mating and territorial
defence. Recent research has shown that the overall intensity of antipredator response and the pattern of threat-sensitive
trade-offs are influenced by current conditions, including variability in predation risk over a period of days to weeks. In
this study, we tested the hypothesis that long-term predation pressure will likewise have shaped the nature of the threat-sensitive
antipredator behaviour of wild-caught Trinidadian guppies (Poecilia reticulata). Female guppies were collected from two populations that have evolved under high- and low-predation pressure, respectively,
in the Aripo River, Northern Mountain Range, Trinidad. Under laboratory conditions, we exposed shoals of three guppies to
varying concentrations of conspecific damage-released chemical alarm cues. Lower Aripo (high-predation) guppies exhibited
the strongest antipredator response when exposed to the highest alarm cue concentration and a graded decline in response intensity
with decreasing concentrations of alarm cue. Upper Aripo (low-predation) guppies, however, exhibited a nongraded (hypersensitive)
response pattern. Our results suggest that long-term predation pressure shapes not only the overall intensity of antipredator
responses of Trinidadian guppies but also their threat-sensitive behavioural response patterns. 相似文献