共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Patrick Gautier Kurtulus Olgun Nazan Uzum Claude Miaud 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2006,59(6):836-841
Gregarious behaviour (i.e. living in groups in contrast to a solitary life) is commonly observed in mammals, but rarely documented
in amphibians. Environmental features and/or animal mutual attractions can promote the formation of aggregations that may
both reduce the risks of dehydration and predation and increase mate access and fitness. Luschan’s salamander (Mertensiella luschani) lives in permanently arid Mediterranean environments; individuals shelter in cracks and crevices and leave only during favourable
periods. In this study we examined the role of chemical tracks, in self and conspecific recognition (i.e. gregarious/solitary
behaviour), on the social structure of this species. Our results show that juveniles and adults of both sexes use chemical
scents deposited on substrate to relocate their shelter. In contrast to numerous other salamander species, Luschan’s salamanders
also use social information, conveyed by conspecific scents, to identify a safe shelter. Furthermore, this scent marking does
not play a role in sexual attraction but allows sex discrimination. This species exhibits gregarious behaviour (i.e. conspecific
attraction) as a possible adaptation to dry environments. We discuss both ultimate and proximate factors in the evolution
from a solitary to a gregarious life. 相似文献
2.
Social structure and co-operative interactions in a wild population of guppies (Poecilia reticulata)
D. P. Croft R. James P. O. R. Thomas C. Hathaway D. Mawdsley K. N. Laland J. Krause 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2006,59(5):644-650
In contrast to the substantial number of theoretical papers that have examined the mechanisms by which cooperation may evolve,
very few studies have investigated patterns of co-operation in natural animal populations. In the current study, we use a
novel approach, social network analysis, to investigate the structure of co-operative interactions in the context of predator
inspection in a wild population of guppies (Poecilia reticulata). Female guppies showed social preferences for stable partners, fulfilling a key assumption made by models of reciprocity.
In the laboratory, wild female guppies disproportionately engaged in predator inspection with others with whom they had strong
social associations. Furthermore, pairs of fish that frequently engaged in predator inspection did so in a particularly co-operative
way, potentially reducing costs associated with predator inspection. Taken together, these results provide evidence for assortative
interactions forming the basis of co-operation during predator inspection in a natural fish population. The occurrence of
highly interconnected social networks between stable partners suggests the existence of co-operation networks in free-ranging
populations of the guppy. 相似文献
3.
Chemical and visual sources of information are used by aquatic prey during risk assessment. Here, we test the behavioral response of littoral prey fish to combinations of chemical alarm cues (skin extract) and the visual presence of a fish shoal. We scented minnow traps with either alarm cues or water (control) placed inside the trap, a jar that contained either a fish shoal or nothing (control), and recorded the number and species of fish captured. We predicted that chemical alarm cues would reduce the number of fish captured and that a fish shoal would increase the number of fish captured. The predicted effect of chemical and visual cues combined depended on the nature of the interaction. We found that the lowest catch rate was for the combination of alarm cue + no shoal, but the highest catch rate occurred for the combination of alarm cue + shoal. Fish shoal + water had the second highest catch rate and no shoal + water had the second lowest catch rate. We conclude that chemical alarm cues induce area avoidance in the absence of a shoal, but a strong behavioral proclivity to increase shoal cohesion in the presence of a shoal. The presence of a shoal in the traps induced alarmed fish to shoal with them and thus, enter the traps. This occurred even though traps were the source of the alarm cue.Communicated by A. Mathis 相似文献
4.
Ashley J. W. Ward Robert I. Holbrook Jens Krause Paul J. B. Hart 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2005,57(6):575-583
The social organisation of animals relies on recognition. However, there are many means by which animals may recognise one another and a variety of cues are available to any individual at any one time. We tested the effects of cues based on direct experience and of cues based on habitat and diet on association decisions in the three-spined stickleback (Gasterosteus aculeatus). Our results show that sticklebacks rapidly acquire association preferences for novel con- and heterospecifics (nine-spined sticklebacks, Pungitius pungitius) which had experienced the same habitat and diet conditioning as themselves over novel con- and heterospecifics which had experienced a different habitat and diet conditioning, a preference which may be based on self-referent matching. Association preferences were observed after only 24 h and were independent of the number of tank mates (treatments with 20 and 100 fish). Evidence for the influence of direct social experience on association preferences was weak and may be confounded by other factors.Communicated by K. Lindström 相似文献
5.
Christine Errard Abraham Hefetz Pierre Jaisson 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2006,59(3):353-363
To investigate the role of template plasticity in shaping nest-mate recognition processes in ants, we constructed experimental
mixed-species groups of Manica rubida with either Myrmica rubra, Tetramorium bicarinatum or Formica selysi. Selecting Ma. rubida as the focal species, we observed the behaviour within mixed-species groups and the transfer rates of cuticular hydrocarbons
(CHC) onto the focal ants, and we also tested the aggression of the focal species reared either alone or in association with
each of the three different species. We show that Ma. rubida workers were always amicable towards their mixed group members, as towards members of the respective parental colonies, irrespective
of the associated species. They did, however, express different levels of aggression towards single-species groups of the
other species tested, depending on the species with which they were reared. The study suggests that similarity in CHC profiles
in two species leads to a narrow template in mixed groups, while dissimilarity is followed by lower levels of aggression (a
broader template), at least against species with similar CHC compound compositions (i.e. both a broader template in the focal
ants and familiarity with the compound groups of the tested individuals operate together). This refutes the hypothesis that
ants reared in mixed-species groups are systematically more tolerant. It also demonstrates that heterospecific information
is not treated equally during development. We suggest that post-imaginal learning, template reforming and decision making
are more precisely tuned when the two species' chemical complexes are similar. 相似文献
6.
We studied the factors that enhance food recognition and consumption in young canaries when confronted with adults. In contrast
to previous studies on canaries, in which social transmission of food habits was studied in the context of dyadic interactions
(one juvenile–one adult), we proposed a more realistic framework in which young canaries were studied in the context of triadic
interactions, free or not, with adults of both sexes. We found that during free interactions, the young bird only eats with
a familiar male and that this association enhances the social transmission of seed handling. When the juvenile was separated
from the adults by a transparent partition, it only learned to husk seed if it was present at the feeder at the same time
as a familiar adult acting as a demonstrator. The presence of adults that are familiar but do not act as demonstrators does
not facilitate social transmission of handling. However, the presence of a familiar, demonstrating female had also no effect
on this transmission. Coordination of the actions of the experienced bird and of the naive subject is required for social
transmission to occur. Action coordination does not depend solely on the level of familiarity between partners but also on
the role played by the demonstrator (here, the adult male) that looks after the juvenile during its transition towards independence. 相似文献
7.
Female association preferences based on olfactory cues in the guppy,<Emphasis Type="Italic"> Poecilia reticulata</Emphasis> 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Guppies are highly sexually dimorphic. Females have been shown to mate preferentially with males with various visible traits and previous studies investigating mate choice have focussed on these visual cues. However, guppies have been shown to respond behaviourally to odour cues from conspecifics as well as from predators. Therefore, sexual selection in guppies may act on multiple sensory modalities, but so far only visual cues have been investigated. Here, we used a specially designed olfactory choice tank to determine whether females could detect other guppies on the basis of their olfactory cues only, and to determine whether females preferred to associate with males or females and whether they could distinguish between different males. We show that female guppies associated preferentially with other females when given a choice between water containing the odour cues from another fish and water containing no odour cues. When females were presented with olfactory cues from a male and a female, they investigated the female initially, but then most reversed their decision and swam to the male. Furthermore, we demonstrate that females associated preferentially with certain males based upon olfactory cues alone, but that this choice was in direct opposition to that made when the females received only visual cues from the same males.Communicated by: M. Abrahams 相似文献
8.
Structure of the social network and its influence on transmission dynamics in a honeybee colony 总被引:4,自引:3,他引:1
Dhruba Naug 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2008,62(11):1719-1725
Infectious processes in a social group are driven by a network of contacts that is generally structured by the organization
arising from behavioral and spatial heterogeneities within the group. Although theoretical models of transmission dynamics
have placed an overwhelming emphasis on the importance of understanding the network structure in a social group, empirical
data regarding such contact structures are rare. In this paper, I analyze the network structure and the correlated transmission
dynamics within a honeybee colony as determined by food transfer interactions and the changes produced in it by an experimental
manipulation. The study demonstrates that widespread transmission in the colony is correlated to a lower clustering coefficient
and higher robustness of the social network. I also show that the social network in the colony is determined by the spatial
distribution of various age classes, and the resulting organizational structure provides some amount of immunity to the young
individuals. The results of this study demonstrates how, using the honeybee colony as a model system, concepts in network
theory can be combined with those in behavioral ecology to gain a better understanding of social transmission processes, especially
those related to disease dynamics. 相似文献
9.
We used interdemic variation in the tendency to form mixed-species groups to examine the costs and benefits of association
among the primates of Kibale National Park, Uganda. A year-long survey of six sites revealed that the amount of time that
the five common diurnal primates [red colobus (Procolobus
tephrosceles), black-and-white colobus (Colobus
guereza), redtail monkeys (Cercopithecus
ascanius), blue monkeys (Cercopithecus
mitis), and grey-cheeked mangabeys (Lophocebus
albigena)] spent in mixed-species groups varied dramatically among sites. In many cases, the proportion of time that species associated
was positively related to their densities. By using detailed behavioral observations of redtail monkeys and red colobus made
over 4 years (2660 h) at four sites, we were able to reject the null hypothesis that associations occur by chance for only
one of four sites. However, a correlative approach exploring the costs and benefits of association suggests that ecological
variables do influence association patterns. We found that redtail monkeys and red colobus overlapped in diet (19.2% of their
foraging effort) and traveled further when in mixed-species groups than when alone. Having demonstrated this, we examined
the applicability of the ecological constraints model for predicting the proportion of the time spent in mixed-species groups
based on food availability. For this analysis we concentrated on red colobus from the site with 35 months of observation and
demonstrated that their tendency to be in mixed- species groups was related to food availability. We used two methods to examine
if mixed-species associations function to decrease predation risk. First, chimpanzees are known to prey heavily on red colobus,
but rarely kill other primates. The time red colobus spent in mixed-species groups was correlated to chimpanzee density, but
it was not for the other monkey species, suggesting that mixed-species groups serve to decrease predation risk. Second, when
red colobus groups contain more infants and are presumably at the greatest risk of predation, they form mixed-species groups
most often. These results demonstrate that the costs and benefits of mixed-species associations vary dramatically over small
spatial and temporal scales. If such variation is generally the case, then studies conducted at different locations or different
times could easily highlight the importance of difference selective agents in favoring mixed-species associations.
Received: 10 February 1999 / Received in revised form: 16 September 1999 / Accepted: 2 October 1999 相似文献
10.
Data on langur (Presbytis entellus) populations were gathered from the literature to test the importance of three selective pressures in determining group size
and composition: predation pressure, intergroup resource defense, and conspecific threat. There were no detectable difference
in the size of groups in populations facing nearly intact predator communities compared to those populations where predators
were severely reduced in number or absent, although there was a trend for the number of adult males per group to increase
in areas with nearly intact predator communities. Using population density as an indirect measure of the frequency of intrusions
into a group’s home range and thereby as an index of the demographic pressure favoring resource defense, we predicted that
higher densities would result in larger defensive coalitions and higher numbers of females per group. This prediction was
not upheld. Our third selective pressure, conspecific threat, encompasses those selective forces resulting from physical attack
on females, infants, and juveniles. Our index of conspecific threat uses the number of non-group males divided by the number
of bisexual groups, because in langurs, the major source of conspecific threat derives from non-group males who, following
group take-over, kill infants, wound females, and expel juveniles from groups. This index of conspecific threat was strongly
related to the mean number of resident females, was weakly related to the mean group size, but was not related to the number
of males in the group. In addition, as predicted, populations with a high index of conspecific threat had higher levels of
juvenile expulsion. These analyses were corroborated by a simulation model which used a computer-generated series of null
populations to calculate expected slopes of immatures regressed on adult females. These randomly generated populations, matched
to means and ranges of real populations, allowed us to determine if deviations of the observed slopes from the expected null
slopes could be explained by variation in predation pressure, population density, or conspecific threat. We found no evidence
that predation pressure was associated with decreases in immature survival in smaller groups, as would be predicted by the
predation-avoidance hypothesis. We found no evidence that immature survival was compromised by small group size in high-density
populations, as would be predicted by the resource-defense hypothesis. However, as the index of conspecific threat increased,
groups with larger numbers of females were more successful than groups with fewer females in reducing mortality or expulsion
of immatures. Overall, conspecific threat received the strongest support as a selective pressure influencing langur group
size and composition, suggesting that this selective pressure should be evaluated more widely as a factor influencing composition
of animal groups.
Received: 23 January 1995/Accepted after revision: 18 February 1996 相似文献
11.
We report on the genetic evaluation and behavioral study of social organization in the Asian elephant (Elephas maximus). Although Asian elephants and African elephants (Loxodonta africana) were previously thought to have similar social organizations, our results demonstrate a substantial difference in the complexity
and structure of Asian elephant social groupings from that described for African savanna elephants. Photographic cataloging
of individuals, radio telemetry, and behavioral observations in Ruhuna National Park, Sri Lanka, enabled us to assign associated
females and young to four groups with overlapping ranges. Genetic sampling of individuals from the four groups in Ruhuna National
Park and three other groups in surrounding areas, conducted through PCR amplification and sequencing of mitochondrial DNA
from dung, supported the matriarchal nature of female groups and the lack of inter-group transfer of females. Behaviorally
and genetically, the identified social groups were best described as ”family groups”. We did not find any evidence for the
existence of social groups of higher complexity than family groups.
Received: 25 March 2000 / Received in revised form: 28 March 2000 / Accepted: 1 April 2000 相似文献
12.
David Goulson Jemma L. Cruise Kate R. Sparrow Adele J. Harris Kirsty J. Park Matthew C. Tinsley Andre S. Gilburn 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2007,61(10):1523-1529
Flowers exhibit great intra-specific variation in the rewards they offer. At any one time, a significant proportion of flowers
often contain little or no reward. Hence, foraging profitably for floral rewards is problematic and any ability to discriminate
between flowers and avoid those that are less rewarding will confer great advantages. In this study, we examine discrimination
by foraging bees among flowers of nasturtium, Tropaeolum majus. Bee visitors included carpenter bees, Xylocopa violacea, which were primary nectar robbers; honeybees, Apis mellifera, which either acted as secondary nectar robbers or gathered pollen legitimately and bumblebees, Bombus hortorum, which were the only bees able to gather nectar legitimately. Many flowers were damaged by phytophagous insects. Nectar volume
was markedly lower in flowers with damaged petals (which were also likely to be older) and in flowers that had nectar-robbing
holes. We test whether bees exhibit selectivity with regards to the individual flowers, which they approach and enter, and
whether this selectivity enhances foraging efficiency. The flowers approached (within 2 cm) by A. mellifera and B. hortorum were non-random when compared to the floral population; both species selectively approached un-blemished flowers. They both
approached more yellow flowers than would be expected by chance, presumably a reflection of innate colour preferences, for
nectar standing crop did not vary according to flower colour. Bees were also more likely to accept (land on) un-blemished
flowers. A. mellifera gathering nectar exhibited selectivity with regards to the presence of robbing holes, being more likely to land on robbed
flowers (they are not able to feed on un-robbed flowers). That they frequently approached un-robbed flowers suggests that
they are not able to detect robbing holes at long-range, so that foraging efficiency may be limited by visual acuity. Nevertheless,
by using a combination of long-range and short-range selectivity, nectar-gathering A. mellifera and B. hortorum greatly increased the average reward from the flowers on which they landed (by 68% and 48%, respectively) compared to the
average standing crop in the flower population. Overall, our results demonstrate that bees use obvious floral cues (colour
and petal blemishes) at long-range, but can switch to using more subtle cues (robbing holes) at close range. They also make
many mistakes and some cues used do not correlate with floral rewards. 相似文献
13.
Michael Hrncir Sidnei Mateus Fábio S. Nascimento 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2007,61(6):975-983
An efficient exploitation of carbohydrate food sources would be beneficial for social wasp species that store nectar within
their nest. In the swarm-founding polistine wasp Polybia occidentalis, we now demonstrate that the decisions of when and where to forage are influenced by information from conspecifics. Only
when foragers had been trained to collect at artificial carbohydrate feeders did newcomers (food-source-naive individuals)
continuously arrive at these feeders during 2 h of experiment. In control tests, in which no forager had been trained, not
a single newcomer alighted at any of the offered carbohydrate food sources. This indicates that, during the foraging process,
a nest-based input provided by successful foragers must have stimulated nestmates to search for food. Once activated, the
newcomers’ choice on where to collect was strongly influenced by field-based social information. The mere visual presence
of accumulated conspecifics (wasp dummies placed on one of the feeders) attracted newcomers to the food sources. Interestingly,
however, visual enhancement was not the only decision-biasing factor at the feeding site. In an experimental series where
searching wasps had to choose between the experimental feeder at which 3 foragers continuously collected and the control feeder
with nine wasp dummies, only 40% of the wasps chose the visually enhanced feeder. This points to the existence of additional
mechanisms of local enhancement. The possibility that, in social wasps, recruitment is involved in the exploitation of carbohydrate
food sources is discussed. 相似文献
14.
Vicarious sampling: the use of personal and public information by starlings foraging in a simple patchy environment 总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6
Group foragers may be able to assess patch quality more efficiently by paying attention to the sampling activities of conspecifics
foraging in the same patch. In a previous field experiment, we showed that starlings foraging on patches of hidden food could
use the successful foraging activities of others to help them assess patch quality. In order to determine whether a starling
could also use another individual’s lack of foraging success to assess and depart from empty patches more quickly, we carried
out two experimental studies which compared the behaviour of captive starlings sampling artificial patches both when alone
and when in pairs. Solitary starlings were first trained to assess patch quality in our experimental two-patch system, and
were then tested on an empty patch both alone and with two types of partner bird. One partner sampled very few holes and thus
provided a low amount of public information; the other sampled numerous holes and thus provided a high amount of public information.
In experiment 1, we found no evidence of vicarious sampling. Subjects sampled a similar number of empty holes when alone as
when with the low and high information partners; thus they continued to rely on their own personal information to make their
patch departure decisions. In experiment 2, we modified the experimental patches, increasing the ease with which a bird could
watch another’s sampling activities, and increasing the difficulty of acquiring accurate personal sampling information. This
time, subjects apparently did use public information, sampling fewer empty holes before departure when with the high-information
partner than when with the low-information partner, and sampling fewer holes when with the low-information partner than when
alone. We suggest that the degree to which personal and public information are used is likely to depend both on a forager’s
ability to remember where it has already sampled and on the type of environment in which foraging takes place.
Received: 31 January 1995/Accepted after revision: 11 September 1995 相似文献
15.
We assessed experimentally how the quality and quantity of social information affected foraging decisions of starlings (Sturnus vulgaris) at different neighbour distances, and how individuals gained social information as a function of head position. Our experimental set up comprised three bottomless enclosures, each housing one individual placed on a line at different distances. The birds in the extreme enclosures were labelled senders and the one in the centre receiver. We manipulated the foraging opportunities of senders (enhanced, natural, no-foraging), and recorded the behaviour of the receiver. In the first experiment, receivers responded to the condition of senders. Their searching rate and food intake increased when senders foraged in enhanced conditions, and decreased in no-foraging conditions, in relation to natural conditions. Scanning was oriented more in the direction of conspecifics when senders behaviour departed from normal. In the second experiment, responses were dose dependent: receivers increased their searching rate and orientated their gaze more towards conspecifics with the number of senders foraging in enhanced food conditions. In no-foraging conditions, receivers decreased their searching and intake rates with the number of senders, but no variation was found in scanning towards conspecifics. Differences in foraging and scanning behaviour between enhanced and no-foraging conditions were much lower when neighbours were separated farther. Overall, information transfer within starling flocks affects individual foraging and scanning behaviour, with receivers monitoring and copying senders behaviour mainly when neighbours are close. Information transfer may be related to predation information (responding to the vigilance of conspecifics) and foraging information (responding to the feeding success of conspecifics). Both sources of information, balanced by neighbour distance, may simultaneously affect the behaviour of individuals in natural conditions.Communicated by H. Kokko 相似文献
16.
Tuan T. Cao Kelly M. Hyland Alana Malechuk Lee A. Lewis Stanley S. Schneider 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2009,63(4):521-529
Communication signals used in animal social interactions are frequently performed repetitively, but the function of this repetition
is often not well understood. We examined the effects of signal repetition by investigating the behavior of worker honey bees
that received differing numbers of vibration signals in established and newly founded colonies, which could use signal repetition
differently to help adjust task allocations to the labor demands associated with the different stages of colony development.
In both colony types, more than half of all monitored workers received more than one vibration signal, and approximately 12%
received ≥5 signals during a given 20-min observation period. Vibrated recipients exhibited greater activity and task performance
than same-age non-vibrated controls at all levels of signal activity. However, vibrated workers showed similar levels of task
performance, movement rates, cell inspection rates, and trophallactic exchanges regardless of the number of signals received.
Thus, the repeated performance of vibration signals on individual bees did not cause cumulative increases in the activity
of certain workers, but rather may have functioned to maintain relatively constant levels of activity and task performance
among groups of recipients. The established and newly founded colonies did not differ in the extent to which individual workers
received the different numbers of vibration signals or in the levels of activity stimulated by repeated signals. Previous
work has suggested that compared to established colonies, newly founded colonies have a greater number of vibrators that perform
signals on a greater proportion of the workers they contact. Taken in concert, these results suggest that vibration signal
repetition may help to adjust task allocations to the different stages of colony development by helping to maintain similar
levels of activity among a greater total number of recipients, rather than by eliciting cumulative effects that cause certain
recipients to work harder than others. 相似文献
17.
Black spots and female association preferences in a sexual/asexual mating complex (Poecilia, Poeciliidae, Teleostei) 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Michael Tobler Martin Plath Heike Burmeister Ingo Schlupp 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2006,60(2):159-165
We investigated whether female association preferences for males are influenced by black spot disease (BSD), a parasite induced change of the host phenotype. We compared three different species of fish: a gynogenetic hybrid species, Poecilia formosa (amazon molly) and two sexual species (Poecilia latipinna and Poecilia mexicana), which were involved in the natural hybridisation leading to the amazon molly. Contrary to their sexual relatives, asexual amazon mollies significantly avoided images of males infected with black spot disease. We propose that amazon molly females have direct fitness benefits from choosing healthy males. The adaptive significance of the preference for BSD-uninfected males in the asexual amazon molly is yet unclear but may involve avoidance of predation or parasite infection as well as increased sperm availability. 相似文献
18.
Plant defense against herbivores often involves constitutive and inducible mechanisms of resistance. Obligate ant-plants, which provide food and housing for ants, are thought to primarily rely on ants for defense against herbivores. This form of plant defense has largely been viewed as static. We have been investigating the dynamic nature of Azteca ants as an inducible defense of Cecropia trees. Ants rapidly recruit to and patrol sites of foliar damage. We propose that Azteca ants can be viewed as an inducible defense for Cecropia trees because of their sensitivity to cues associated with herbivory, their rapid and aggressive recruiting ability, and their reclaimable and redeployable nature as a plant defense. In this study, we examine ant behavior following plant damage, and the potential cues that indude ant recruitment. We found that ants present on leaves when the plant is damaged leave the damaged leaf and recruit other ants to it, presumably by laying recruitment trails. Volatile leaf cues associated with herbivory were important in eliciting an induced response in two experiments. However, we found that cues associated with a congeneric plant elicited a much stronger ant response than conspecific cues. Although the type of leaf damage (gaping wounds versus leaf edge wounds) did not affect the level of ant recruitment, the extent of damage did. Leaves with one hole punched showed a 50% increase in ants, while leaves with five holes punched in them elicited a 100% increase in ant numbers. In sum, it appears that multiple plant-related cues associated with herbivory are involved in induction of ant recruitment in the Cecropia-Azteca system. We discuss the generality of ant responses to herbivory in obligate ant-plant systems, and in facultative ant-plant associations, which may be more common. Received: 23 March 1998 / Accepted after revision: 5 July 1998 相似文献
19.
Socially dominant males often signal their status to rival males and/or females. We tested the hypotheses that Lacerta monticola femoral gland secretions and copulatory plugs convey chemical information about male identity and dominance status. We estimated male dominance status by staging male–male agonistic encounters in a neutral arena. We then conducted two experiments to compare male tongue-flick behavior toward chemical stimuli consisting of cotton swabs bearing (1) deionized water (control), the lizard’s own femoral secretions, and the femoral secretions of another male and (2) phosphate-buffered saline solution (control), the lizard’s own plug products, and the plug products of another male. Results indicate that males discriminated their own femoral secretions and plugs from those of other males. They also discriminated morphological attributes of other males that were associated with dominance status based on chemical cues arising from femoral secretions and discriminated the dominance status of other males based on chemical cues arising from the plugs. Femoral secretions that convey information about male identity and dominance status may be hypothesized to function in the establishment of L. monticola dominance hierarchies through scent-marking of territories. We suggest that copulatory plugs and femoral secretions may allow males to scent-mark the female body and postulate that this behavior may influence male and female reproductive decisions under selective pressures of sperm competition. 相似文献
20.
The objective of our study was to investigate the spatial distribution and genetic structure of a solitary primate at the microgeographical scale of adjacent local populations. We obtained spatial data and tissue samples for mtDNA analysis from 205 gray mouse lemurs (Microcebus murinus) captured along transects and within 3 grid systems within a 12.3 km2 area in Kirindy Forest, western Madagascar. Our capture data revealed that, even though the forest was continuous, gray mouse lemurs were not evenly distributed, and that daily and maximum dispersal distances were significantly greater in males. The frequency distribution of 22 mtDNA D-loop haplotypes was highly skewed. Nine haplotypes were unique to males, indicating male-mediated gene flow from surrounding areas. The geographic distribution of haplotypes revealed that males were also more dispersed than females. Females with the same haplotype showed a tendency towards spatial aggregation, and the correlation between genetic and geographic distances was higher in females. In several areas of the forest, however, spatially clustered females were not of the same haplotype, and females were not always found in clusters. Hence, in contrast to suggestions from previous studies, matrilineal clustering is not the only way females are socially organized. In addition, our study revealed heterogeneity and patterns in population structure that were not evident at smaller spatial scales, some of which may be relevant for designing conservation strategies.Communicated by C. Nunn 相似文献