共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
The impact of copper exposure on Chinese toad (Bufo gargarizans) tadpoles was investigated in this study. First, the 96h LC50 value of copper was 8.697 μM, by means of a 4 d acute toxicity test. Second, we studied the chronic effects of copper on B. gargarizans tadpoles at control, 0.025, 0.1 and 1.0 μM concentration. Survival, body length, body weight, developmental stage, incidence of metamorphic climax, and size at metamorphic climax were determined. In tadpoles developed to metamorphic climax (stage G42), liver and thyroid gland were assessed histologically. Copper at 0.1 and 1.0 μM could inhibit tadpole growth and prolong tadpole metamorphic progress relative to controls. Tadpole size (total length and weight) at stage G42 is also affected in the 0.1 and 1.0 μM treatments. In addition, histological examinations have revealed that 1.0 μM copper could cause significant pathological changes and hepatocytes degeneration in liver. Furthermore, histomorphological measurements indicated that copper at 0.1 and 1.0 μM reduced thyroid gland size, diameter and number of follicle. In conclusion, our study suggests that Cu could damage the liver and thyroid gland, so growth and metamorphosis of B. gargarizans tadpoles were inhibited resulted of disrupting liver metabolism and THs homeostasis. 相似文献
2.
Predators can have significant nonlethal effects on prey by modifying prey behaviour through chemically mediated interactions.
We examined behavioural responses of wood frog (Ranasylvatica) and American toad (Bufoamericanus) tadpoles to both direct and indirect chemical signals associated with a predatory odonate (Anaxjunius). In laboratory trials, tadpoles of both species responded strongly to water conditioned with Anax nymphs by decreasing foraging rates, becoming immobile, and moving away from the stimulus. The responses to water conditioned
with starved Anax versus Anax that fed on conspecific tadpoles did not differ significantly; these results suggest that tadpoles rely primarily on direct
signals to detect odonates. Rana did not respond to water conditioned with conspecific tissue extracts, while Bufo responded with behaviours that were indistinguishable from those of tadpoles exposed to Anax chemicals. In a field experiment, the responses of R. sylvatica tadpoles to Anax chemicals were similar to those of tadpoles observed in the laboratory. Collectively, our data indicate that tadpoles of
both species use chemical cues to assess predation risk from other community members. Tadpoles can selectively distinguish
members who pose a threat, and only evacuate food patches or reduce foraging rates when in danger. These behaviours appear
to be adaptive and are consistent with the predictions of optimality theory.
Received: 1 August 1997 / Accepted after revision: 22 November 1997 相似文献
3.
Summary The ontogeny of sibling recognition behavior was studied in the laboratory in tadpoles of the western toad (Bufo boreas boreas) to test the hypothesis that Bufo tadpoles associate with siblings and to compare this behavior with two species of anurans previously studied. Tadpoles reared exclusively with sibs demonstrated a preference to associate with sibs over non-sibs both early and late in development but tadpoles reared with sibs and non-sibs (mixed rearing groups) exhibited no preference. Larvae that developed a preference for sibs after being reared with them for 75 days lost this preference following exposure to a mixed group for 2 to 6 days. Additionally, larvae reared in a mixed group did not develop a preference for the familiar mixed group nor was a preference exhibited by individual larvae that were reared exclusively with 5 non-sibs. These results suggest that larvae learn by prior association to discriminate sibs from non-sibs and that preferences are rapidly modifiable following exposure to non-sibs. However, the latter two experiments suggest that social preferences are not totally labile and are not based entirely on familiarity. Field observations of larval activity, aggregation behavior, and dispersal patterns in B. boreas indicate that tadpoles mix with non-sibs from early larval stages throughout development. Because even short term exposure to nonsibs resulted in a loss of sib preference in our experiments, we suggest that a kin selection interpretation of B. boreas aggregation behavior is not parsimonious. Results differ with those of other studies of sib recognition in larval anurans. 相似文献
4.
1. | Five species of emballonurid bats (Rhynchonycteris naso, Saccopteryx leptura, Balantiopteryx plicata, Saccopteryx bilineata, and Peropteryx kappleri), were studied in Costa Rica and Trinidad. Stomach contents suggest that prey size generally increases for bat body size, but within these species there is considerable overlap. R. naso, S. leptura, and P. kappleri each appear to be specialized for foraging in a particular habitat type; B. plicata and S. bilineata are more opportunistic and feed over a variety of habitats during the year. While the other species feed in the proximity of surfaces, B. plicata is further separated from the other species by wing specializations favoring high altitude flight. |
2. | Foraging dispersion is more closely related to body size than it is to social structure at the roost: small bats group-forage while larger bats feed in solitary beats. In all of the species, food is spatially and temporally variable, and the location of foraging sites changes seasonally in accordance with these locally varying patterns of aerial insect abundance. In the case of S. bilineata, the locations of foraging sites were positively correlated with levels of phenological activity in the underlying plant communities. |
3. | Colony sizes ranged from small groups of 2–10 bats (S. leptura, P. kappleri), to intermediate colonies of 5–50 bats (R. naso, S. bilineata), to very large colonies with hundreds of bats (B. plicata). |
4. | R. naso, S. leptura, and S. bilineata colonies have colony-specific annual foraging ranges which are actively defended against conspecifics from other colonies. In most cases, all members of a given colony of one of these species will be found foraging in a common site at any time. In R. naso and S. bilineata, currently used foraging sites are partitioned socially. In the former species, adult breeding females occupy a central area and groupforage while younger non-breeding females and males occupy peripheral foraging areas in the colony territory. In S. bilineata, the colony foraging site is partitioned into individual harem territories defended by harem males and containing the individual beats of all current harem females. For this latter species, details of roost site subdivision are mapped directly onto foraging dispersions. In general, there is a close correlation between dayroost group membership and location of nocturnal foraging sites in all of the study species. |
5.
6.
Peter B. Stacey 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1986,18(3):175-187
Summary I studied the foraging behaviour of adults in three different-sized groups of yellow baboons (Papio cynocephalus) at Amboseli National Park in Kenya to assess the relationship between group size and foraging efficiency in this species. Study groups ranged in size from 8 to 44 members; within each group, I collected feeding data for the dominant adult male, the highest ranking pregnant female, and the highest ranking female with a young infant. There were no significant differences between groups during the study in either the mean estimated energy value of the food ingested per day for each individual (385±27 kJ kg-1 day-1) or in the estimated energy expended to obtain that food (114±3 kJ kg-1 day-1). Mean foraging efficiency ratios, which reflect net energy gain per unit of foraging time, also did not vary as a function of the size of the group in which the baboons were living. There was substantial variation between days in the efficiency ratios of all animals; this was the result of large differences in energy intake rather than in the energy expended during foraging itself. The members of the smallest group spent on the average only one-half as much time feeding each day as did individuals in the two larger groups. However, they obtained almost as much energy while foraging, primarily because their rate of food intake while actually eating tended to be higher than the rate in the other groups. The baboons in the small group were observed closer to trees that they could climb to escape ground predators, and they also were more likely to sit in locations elevated above the ground while resting. Such differences would be expected if the members of the small group were less able to detect approaching predators than individuals that lived in the larger groups. The results of this study suggest that predator detection or avoidance, rather than increased foraging efficiency, may be the primary benefit of living in larger groups in this population. 相似文献
7.
Females across many taxa commonly use multiple or complex traits to choose mates. However, the functional significance of
multiple or complex signals remains controversial and largely unknown. Different elements of multiple or complex signals may
convey independent pieces of information about different aspects of a prospective mate (the “multiple messages” hypothesis).
Alternatively, multiple or complex signals could provide redundant information about the same aspect of a prospective mate
(the “redundant” or “back-up” signal hypothesis). We investigated these alternatives using spadefoot toads, Scaphiopus couchii. Spadefoot toads primarily use calls to attract their mates, but males also exhibit sexually dimorphic coloration. We investigated
whether male coloration is indicative of male size, condition, or infection status by a socially transmitted monogenean flatworm.
We found that male coloration and dorsal patterning predicts male size and condition but not infection status. Moreover, when
we presented females with a choice between a bright male model and a dark male model, we found that females preferred the
bright model. Because aspects of males’ calls are also associated with male size and condition, we conclude that coloration
is a potentially redundant indicator of male phenotype. We suggest that coloration could enhance mate choice in conjunction
with male calling behavior by providing females with a long distance cue that could enable them to identify prospective mates
in a noisy chorus environment where the discrimination of individual calls is often difficult. Generally, such redundant signals
may facilitate mate choice by enhancing the quality and accuracy of information females receive regarding prospective mates. 相似文献
8.
Emeline Pettex Svein-H?kon Lorentsen David Grémillet Olivier Gimenez Robert T. Barrett Jean-Baptiste Pons Céline Le Bohec Francesco Bonadonna 《Marine Biology》2012,159(12):2743-2756
The survival of marine predators depends on behavioural plasticity to cope with changes in prey distribution. Variability in behaviour might predict plasticity and is easier to assess than plasticity. Using miniaturized GPS loggers over several breeding seasons in two Norwegian Northern gannet (Morus bassanus) colonies, we investigated if and how the variability within and between individuals, but also between colonies and years, affected foraging strategies. Results revealed strong individual variability (foraging trip durations, foraging effort and different foraging areas). Individuals from both colonies showed preferred commuting routes, flight bearings and feeding hotspots. Individuals from the largest colony used larger and more foraging areas than individuals from the small colony. Feeding hotspots and foraging ranges varied amongst years in the largest colony only. Our study demonstrated that gannets show flexibility by changing prey fields that are driven by shifting oceanographic conditions. 相似文献
9.
Michael Simone-Finstrom Joel Gardner Marla Spivak 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2010,64(10):1609-1617
Honeybees harvest and use plant resins in a mixture called propolis to seal cracks and smooth surfaces in the nest architecture.
Resins in the nest may be important in maintaining a healthy colony due to their antimicrobial properties. This study had
two main objectives: (1) Provide initial insight on the learning capabilities of resin foraging honeybees; (2) analyze the
sensitivity of resin foraging honeybees to tactile stimuli to elucidate its possible role as a mechanism behind resin foraging.
The first objective provides insight into the phenotype of these bees as compared to other forager types, while the second
creates a starting point for further work on behavioral mechanisms of resin foraging. Using tactile proboscis extension response
conditioning, we found that resin foragers learned to associate two different tactile stimuli, the presence of a gap between
two plates and a rough sandpaper surface, with a sucrose reward significantly better than pollen foragers. The results of
differential tactile conditioning exhibited no significant difference in the ability of resin foragers to discriminate between
smooth and rough surfaces as compared to pollen foragers. We also determined that the sucrose response thresholds (SRTs) of
returning resin foragers were lower compared to returning pollen foragers, but both resin foragers and pollen foragers learned
a floral odor equally well. This is the first study to examine SRTs and conditioning to tactile and olfactory stimuli with
resin foraging honeybees. The results provide new information and identify areas for future research on resin collectors,
an understudied foraging phenotype. 相似文献
10.
Chemical signals in bumble bee foraging 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Sydney A. Cameron 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1981,9(4):257-260
Summary Foraging bumblebees (Bombus vosnesenskii) deposit a substance on rewarding flowers which assists in discrimination between rewarding and nonrewarding flowers in a controlled laboratory environment. Discrimination occurs while the bee is on a flower; workers probe rewarding flowers as well as empty ones that have rewarded in the recent past, but they do not probe flowers that have had no reward. Recognition is not the result of honey contamination left on the flower by the bee during feeding. The deposit is only slightly soluble in water or ethyl alcohol but is very soluble in pentane. 相似文献
11.
Competition affects risk-sensitivity in foraging shrews 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Summary Studies of risk-sensitive foraging have so far focused only on the effect of food demand on choice of feeding site. We suggest that competition is likely to be another factor influencing risksensitivity. A choice experiment with common shrews showed that, in the absence of competition, risk-aversion increased with increasing food intake relative to requirement. When apparent competitors were present, however, shrews were risk-indifferent regardless of their estimated requirement. The switch to risk-indifference in the presence of competitors appears to be an all-or-nothing rule of thumb which is not modified by experience with reward probability distributions. 相似文献
12.
Predator and prey spatial distributions have important population and community level consequences. However, little is known either theoretically or empirically about behavioral mechanisms that underlie the spatial patterns that emerge when predators and prey freely interact. We examined the joint space use and behavioral rules governing movement of freely interacting groups of odonate (dragonfly) predators and two size classes of anuran (tadpole) prey in arenas containing two patches with different levels of the prey's resource. Predator and prey movement and space use was quantified both when they were apart and together. When apart from predators, large tadpoles strongly preferred the high resource patch. When apart from prey, dragonflies weakly preferred the high resource patch. When together, large prey shifted to a uniform distribution, while predators strongly preferred the high resource patch. These patterns qualitatively fit the predictions of several three trophic level, ideal free distribution models. In contrast, the space use of small prey and predators did not deviate from uniform. Three measures of joint space use (spatial correlations, overlap, and co-occurrence) concurred in suggesting that prey avoidance of predators was more important than predator attraction to prey in determining overall spatial patterns. To gain additional insight into behavioral mechanisms, we used a model selection approach to identify behavioral movement rules that can potentially explain the observed, emergent patterns of space use. Prey were more likely to leave patches with more predators and more conspecific competitors; resources had relatively weak effects on prey movements. In contrast, predators were more likely to leave patches with low resources (that they do not consume) and more competing predators; prey had relatively little effect on predator movements. These results highlight the importance of investigating freely interacting predators and prey, the potential for simple game theory models to predict joint spatial distributions, and the utility of using model choice methods to identify potential key factors that govern movement. 相似文献
13.
Nina Peuhkuri 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1998,43(4-5):333-337
Individuals which deviate from the majority in groups are likely to be most vulnerable to predation. This oddity effect,
by definition, is frequency dependent, eventually fading at equal frequencies of the phenotypes in a group. It has been hypothesized
that the increased predation risk of odd individuals may play an important role in the formation of phenotypically uniform
shoals of fish. However, recent work has indicated that individuals may experience, or value, their predation hazard differently
depending on their own size in relation to that of other group members: single large fish, but not small ones, appear concerned
about their oddity in a shoal. Here I show that the apparent wariness of large fish is also expressed in a frequency-dependent
manner, closely conforming to what is predicted if the oddity effect is responsible for their behavior. Using foraging activity
of individuals as a means to evaluate their predation risk, I demonstrate with shoals comprising 12 threespine sticklebacks
(Gasterosteus aculeatus) that large fish forage least actively when in a shoal consisting of 2 large and 10 small fish. An increase in the number
of large fish to 4 among 8 small individuals clearly results in an increase in their foraging activity. However, having reached
an equal frequency with small fish in a shoal, large fish do not seem to change their foraging activity much even when their
number in a shoal increases further. In contrast, foraging activity of small sticklebacks remains fairly constant throughout
the entire range of tested shoal compositions, providing further evidence that small and large fish respond to their oddity
differently.
Received: 12 February 1998 / Accepted after revision: 7 May 1998 相似文献
14.
Nancy G. Wolf 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1987,21(1):59-63
Summary Field observations of individuallytagged ocean surgeonfish (Acanthurus bahianus) indicated that schooling provided a foraging benefit: an increase in relative forging time. The magnitude of this increase differed among individuals, primarily due to time budgets of the fish when they were not in schools. The proportion of nonschooling time devoted to foraging was positively correlated with fish size, while the proportion of schooling time that was spent forging was similar for all individuals. Schooling tendency (percentage of time in schools) was positively related to magnitude of foraging benefit and negatively related to fish size. When fish size was held constant by partial correlation, schooling tendency remained positively related to foraging benefit. These results provide evidence that fish which derive a greater foraging benefit from school membership spend more time in schools. 相似文献
15.
Seasonally breeding predators, which are limited in the time available for provisioning young at a central location, and
by the fasting abilities of the young, are likely to maximize energy delivery to the young by maximizing the rate of energy
delivery averaged over the whole period of investment. Reduction in food availability or increased foraging costs will alter
the optimal behavior of individuals. This study examined the behavioral adaptations of a diving predator, the Antarctic fur
seal, to increased foraging costs during lactation. One group of mothers (n=5, treatment) was fitted with additional drag to increase the cost of transport in comparison with a control group (n=8). At the scales of the individual dives, the treatment group made more shorter, shallower (< 30 m) dives. Compensation for
slower swimming speeds was achieved by diving at a steeper angle. Overall, diving behavior conformed to several specific theoretical
predictions but there were also departures from theory, particularly concerning swimming speed during diving. Diving behavior
appears to be adjusted to maximize the proportion of time spent at the bottom of dives. At the scale of diving bouts, no difference
was observed between the treatment and control groups in terms of the frequency and duration of bouts and there was also no
difference between the two groups in terms of the proportion of time spent diving. At the scale of complete foraging cycles,
time taken to return to the pup was significantly longer in the treatment group but there was no difference in the rate of
delivery of energy (measured from pup growth rate) to the pups in each group. Since mothers in the treatment group did not
use significantly more body reserves, we conclude that behavioral adjustments at the scale of individual dives allowed mothers
in the treatment group to compensate for the additional foraging costs. Pup growth rate appears to be less sensitive to the
foraging conditions experienced by mothers than foraging trip duration.
Received: 14 June 1996 / Accepted after revision: 16 November 1996 相似文献
16.
Mathieu Lihoreau Jean-Louis Deneubourg Colette Rivault 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2010,64(10):1577-1587
Group foraging by eusocial insects implies sophisticated recruitment processes that often result in collective decisions to exploit the most profitable sources. These advanced levels of cooperation, however, remain limited to a small range of species, and we still know little about the mechanisms underlying group foraging behaviours in the great mass of animals exhibiting lower levels of social complexity. In this paper, we report, for the first time in a gregarious insect, the cockroach Blattella germanica (L.), a collective foraging decision whereby the selection of food sources is reached without requiring active recruitment. Groups of cockroaches given a binary choice between identical food sources exhibited exploitation asymmetries whose amplitude increases with group size. By coupling behavioural observations to computer simulations, we demonstrate that selection of food sources relies uniquely on a retention effect of feeding individuals on newcomers without comparison between available opportunities. This self-organised pattern presents similarities with the foraging dynamics of eusocial species, thus stressing the generic dimension of collective decision-making mechanisms based on social amplification rules despite fundamental differences in recruitment processes. We hypothesise that such parsimony could apply to a wide range of species and help understand the emergence of collective behaviours in simple social systems. 相似文献
17.
High concentrations of metals (cadmium, copper, lead, and zinc) and of lindane were measured in the sediment and in the spawn and tadpoles of Bufo bufo, Rana dalmatina, and Rana ridibunda from two Austrian locations. Residues in spawn suggest maternal transfer. Increase of the metal concentrations from spawn to advanced tadpole stage is explained by the larval microphaguous feeding habits which cause high exposition of the tadpoles to substances concentrating in sediments and suspended particles. Metal and pesticide concentrations in spawn and tadpoles are toxic to various other aquatic organisms and are furthermore considered to be potentially hazardous to the anurans themselves. The results are conform with world‐wide observations of contamination of anuran larvae with heavy metals, and prove the significance of non‐point source chemical exposure due to allochthonous toxicant input by wind load and precipitation. 相似文献
18.
Summary. In amphibians and fishes, evidence is increasing that chemical cues from injured conspecifics can play a role in the chemical
labelling and learned recognition of unfamiliar predators. In this laboratory study, we tested the prediction that prior chemical
exposure to a non-native predator feeding on conspecific tadpoles will subsequently allow tadpoles of the common toad (Bufo bufo) to recognize the chemical cues specifically released by this starved predator. Furthermore, we investigated the vulnerability
of this chemically-mediated process to herbicide contamination. With these aims in view, groups of tadpoles were kept either
unexposed or exposed for ten days to chemical cues from Turkish crayfish (Astacus leptodactylus) previously fed on tadpoles, both in uncontaminated water and in the presence of four sublethal concentrations of amitrole
(0.01, 0.1, 1 and 10 mg.l−1). We then assessed the effects of the six conditioning treatments on general activity and behavioural response to chemical
cues from starved crayfish. Larval treatments did not affect the general activity of the tadpoles. By contrast, the treatments
had significant effects on the behavioural response to the test solution prepared form starved crayfish. The only tadpoles
to show an antipredator behavioural response to the chemical stimulation from starved crayfish belonged to the groups derived
from chemical exposure to tadpole-fed crayfish in uncontaminated water and in contaminated water with the lowest concentration
of amitrole (0.01 mg.l−1). Conversely, this chemical stimulation produced no behavioural change in the control group or in the groups derived from
exposure to tadpole-fed crayfish in contaminated water containing 0.1, 1 and 10 mg.l−1 of amitrole. This study demonstrates that chemical cues released during the predator’s feeding activity can subsequently
be used by common toad tadpoles in the recognition of an unfamiliar predator. In addition, our results show that the presence
of sublethal amitrole concentrations can impair this recognition process. Such a pesticide effect might be especially detrimental
for amphibian populations threatened by invasive predators. 相似文献
19.
Memory dynamics and foraging strategies of honeybees 总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6
Summary The foraging behavior of a single bee in a patch of four electronic flower dummies (feeders) was studied with the aim of analyzing the informational components in the choice process. In different experimental combinations of reward rates, color marks, odors and distances of the feeders, the behavior of the test bee was monitored by a computer in real time by several devices installed in each feeder. The test bee optimizes by partially matching its choice behavior to the reward rates of the feeders. The matching behavior differs strongly between stay flights (the bee chooses the feeder just visited) and shift flights (the bee chooses one of the three alternative feeders). The probability of stay and shift flights depends on the reward sequence and on the time interval between successive visits. Since functions describing the rising probability of stay flights with rising amounts of sucrose solution just experienced differ for the four feeders, it is concluded that bees develop feeder-specific memories. The choice profiles of shift flights between the three alternative feeders depend on the mean reward rate of the feeder last visited. Good matching is found after visits to the low-reward feeders and poor matching following departure from the high-reward feeders. These results indicate that bees use two different kinds of memories to guide their choice behavior: a transient short-term working memory that is not feeder-specific, and a feeder-specific long-term reference memory. Model calculations were carried out to test this hypothesis. The model was based on a learning rule (the difference rule) developed by Rescorla and Wagner (1972), which was extended to the two forms of memories to predict this operant behavior. The experiments show that a foraging honeybee learns the properties of a food source (its signals and rewards) so effectively that specific expectations guide the choice behavior.
Correspondence to: R. Menzel 相似文献
20.
Simon Garnier Aurélie Guérécheau Maud Combe Vincent Fourcassié Guy Theraulaz 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2009,63(8):1167-1179
We experimentally investigated both individual and collective behavior of the Argentine ant Linepithema humile as they crossed symmetrical and asymmetrical bifurcations in gallery networks. Ants preferentially followed the branch that
deviated the least from their current direction and their probability to perform a U-turn after a bifurcation increased with
the turning angle at the bifurcation. At the collective level, colonies were better able to find the shortest path that linked
the nest to a food source in a polarized network where bifurcations were symmetrical from one direction and asymmetrical from
the other than in a network where all bifurcations were symmetrical. We constructed a model of individual behavior and showed
that an individual’s preference for the least deviating path will be amplified via the ants’ mass recruitment mechanism thus
explaining the difference found between polarized and non-polarized networks. The foraging efficiency measured in the simulations
was three times higher in polarized than in non-polarized networks after only 15 min. We conclude that measures of transport
network efficiency must incorporate both the structural properties of the network and the behavior of the network users. 相似文献