共查询到16条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
We report on the genetic evaluation and behavioral study of social organization in the Asian elephant (Elephas maximus). Although Asian elephants and African elephants (Loxodonta africana) were previously thought to have similar social organizations, our results demonstrate a substantial difference in the complexity
and structure of Asian elephant social groupings from that described for African savanna elephants. Photographic cataloging
of individuals, radio telemetry, and behavioral observations in Ruhuna National Park, Sri Lanka, enabled us to assign associated
females and young to four groups with overlapping ranges. Genetic sampling of individuals from the four groups in Ruhuna National
Park and three other groups in surrounding areas, conducted through PCR amplification and sequencing of mitochondrial DNA
from dung, supported the matriarchal nature of female groups and the lack of inter-group transfer of females. Behaviorally
and genetically, the identified social groups were best described as ”family groups”. We did not find any evidence for the
existence of social groups of higher complexity than family groups.
Received: 25 March 2000 / Received in revised form: 28 March 2000 / Accepted: 1 April 2000 相似文献
2.
Common shrews (Sorex araneus) maintain a foraging territory for most of their immature life. Possessing a high-quality territory is vital for overwinter
survival in the harsh boreal climate, and hence, competitive ability in territorial disputes is expected to be an important
component of individual fitness. To test possible association between individual inbreeding and fitness, we used neutral arena
trials to assess the competitive performance of young common shrews. The experiment involved pairs of individuals originating
from small island populations, where breeding must often occur between related individuals, and from large outbred mainland
populations. The percentage of neutral arena tests that an individual won was highly significantly explained by internal relatedness,
a surrogate measure of individual inbreeding, measured using ten microsatellite markers. Body size, sex, learning, and population
type (mainland vs island) made no significant contributions. Even a low level of individual inbreeding may lead to significant
adverse consequences in multiple territorial contests, which may represent a significant cause of inbreeding depression in
many wild vertebrate populations. 相似文献
3.
繁殖期白头鹤觅食地活动的时间分配和日节律 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
2004年4~6月在黑龙江大沾河湿地自然保护区内,采用野外直接观察法对野生白头鹤在觅食地的活动节律进行了研究.结果表明:①繁殖期白头鹤在觅食地各种行为活动有一定的时间分配规律.在繁殖前期,雌、雄白头鹤全天(5:30~18:00)成对在觅食地活动,雄性个体以取食、警戒、游走、保养和求偶为主要行为,雌性个体以取食、警戒、游走和保养为主要行为;到繁殖后期,雌、雄白头鹤每天有两个时间段单独在觅食地活动(4:00~6:00和17:00~19:00),无论雌雄个体,均无求偶行为.②白头鹤雌、雄个体间保养行为差异极显著(P<0.001),警戒和求偶行为差异显著(P<0.05),取食、游走行为具有一定的差异.③在麦田和池塘2种微地貌之间,白头鹤的取食、求偶行为具极显著差异(P<0.001),警戒行为具显著差异(P<0.05).④在繁殖期的不同阶段以及不同的天气情况下白头鹤的各种行为均具有一定差异.图1表2参15 相似文献
4.
Filippo Galimberti Simona Sanvito Chiara Braschi Luigi Boitani 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2007,62(2):159-171
Reproductive effort is a key parameter of life history because it measures the resources allocated to reproduction at the
expense of growth and maintenance. Male reproductive effort always had a minor role with respect to female effort both in
the development of theories and in field research. Elephant seals are an ideal subject for reproductive effort studies because
they fast during the breeding season, splitting the phase of energy acquisition from the phase of energy use for breeding.
In this paper, we present results on male reproductive effort (weight loss estimated by photogrammetry) in southern elephant
seals (Mirounga leonina), the most dimorphic and polygynous of all mammal species. We show that total reproductive effort increases with age, with
no sign of late decrease or senescence. Male reproductive effort in this species depends mostly on behavioral factors, i.e.,
the success in competition with other males, and the intensity of interaction with females. A large effort results in large
gains in both mating success and fertilizations. The large reproductive success that a few males are able to achieve come
at a big cost in terms of energy expenditure, but this cost does not seem to affect the likelihood of survival to the following
breeding season. 相似文献
5.
Social insect colonies can be expected to forage at rates that maximize colony fitness. Foraging at higher rates would increase the rate of worker production, but decrease adult survival. This trade-off has particular significance during the founding stage, when adults lost are not replaced. Prior work has shown that independent-founding wasps rear the first workers rapidly by foraging at high rates. Foraging rates decrease after those individuals pupate, presumably reducing the risk of foundress death. In the swarm-founding wasps, colony-founding units have many workers, making colony death by forager attrition less likely. Do swarm-founding wasps show similar shifts in foraging rates during the founding stage? We measured foraging rates of the swarm-founding wasp, Polybia occidentalis at four stages of colony development. At each stage, foraging rates correlated with the number of larvae present, which, in the founding stages, correlated with the number of cells in the new nest. Thus, foraging rates appear to be demand-driven, with the level of demand in the founding stage set by the size of nest that is constructed. During the founding stage, foraging rates per larva were high initially, suggesting that colonies minimize the development times of larvae early in the founding stage. Later in the stage, foraging rates decreased, which would reduce worker mortality until new workers eclose. This pattern is similar to that shown for independent-founding wasps and likely results from conflicting pressures to maximize colony growth and minimize the risk of colony death by forager attrition. 相似文献
6.
Bumblebees (Bombus terrestris) are attracted to those particular inflorescences where other bees are already foraging, a process known as local enhancement.
Here, we use a quantitative analysis of learning in a foraging task to illustrate that this attraction can lead bees to learn
more quickly which flower species are rewarding if they forage in the company of experienced conspecifics. This effect can
also be elicited by model bees, rather than live demonstrators. We also show that local enhancement in bumblebees most likely
reflects a general attraction to conspecifics that is not limited to a foraging context. Based on the widespread occurrence
of both local enhancement and associative learning in the invertebrates, we suggest that social influences on learning in
this group may be more common than the current literature would suggest and that invertebrates may provide a useful model
for understanding how learning processes based on social information evolve. 相似文献
7.
The grand skink, Oligosoma grande, is a diurnal rock-dwelling lizard from the tussock grasslands of Central Otago, New Zealand, whose diet includes a variety
of arthropods and fruit. We conducted a field experiment to examine the influence of prey distribution on foraging behavior
and spacing patterns. On sites where prey distribution was unaltered (control sites), males and females differed in diet and
foraging behavior. Most male feeding attempts were directed at large strong-flying insects, and males used a saltatory search
pattern that involved relatively infrequent moves of long duration. Females spent more effort catching small weak-flying insects
and visiting fruiting plants. Their search behavior involved frequent moves of short duration. The placement of meat-bait
on experimental sites led to a redistribution of large flies without influencing other prey types. Experimental females switched
foraging strategy by adopting a search pattern of relatively infrequent moves of long duration, increasing the frequency of
attempts to capture large prey, and reducing the importance of fruit in their diet. The experimental manipulation appeared
to influence space use. On control sites, both sexes had comparably sized home ranges. On experimental sites, male home ranges
were significantly larger than female home ranges.
Received: 3 November 1997 / Accepted after revision: 13 December 1998 相似文献
8.
Summary Two forms of lingual protrusion, tongueflicking and labial-licking, were differentially affected by combinations of movement and eating conditions in a eublepharid gecko (Eublepharis macularius). Tongue-flicking, in which the tongue contacts substrates beyond the lizard's body, occurred at increased rates during locomotion and during locomotion was significantly more frequent after eating than in a baseline condition. Labial-licking, in which a protruded portion of the tongue touches the labial, mental or rostral scales that surround the mouth, increased after eating. Unlike tongue-flick rates, by far the highest labial-lick rates were observed in stationary lizards after eating. The elevated tongue-flicking rates during movement after eating may be a manifestation of a postingestive chemosensory search for prey. In addition to grooming, several possible chemosensory functions of labial-licking are discussed, including gustatory sampling, sampling prey chemicals on the labials for transfer to the vomeronasal system, and redistribution of chemicals on the tongue to enhance transfer. It is suggested that labial-licking might help motionless lizards maintain vigilance for visual prey stimuli associated with the specific chemical prey cues. Another possible explanation for the increased labial-lick rate while motionless after eating is that prey chemicals induce tongue-flicking, but that the distance protruded is lessened and the tongue does not contact environmental substrates. Tongue-flicking while stationary is unlikely to lead to detection of additional prey and might incur detection by the lizard's predators or prey. 相似文献
9.
When feeding on blue mussels (Mytilus edulis), oystercatchers (Haematopus ostralegus) either stab into the mollusc’s gaping valves or hammer through its dorsal or ventral shell. Whilst the selectivity of hammering
and stabbing oystercatchers for specific prey morphologies has been well studied, the way in which the effects of environment
on M.
edulis morphology can in turn affect feeding methods of H. ostralegus is very poorly understood. Based on morphological analyses on randomly selected shells from three intertidal zones, this
study failed to detect differences in morphology or distribution of dorsally and ventrally hammered shells but confirms the
finding of previous authors that hammering oystercatchers select thinner mussels than stabbing birds. Additionally, we show
that this difference in optimal prey morphology can lead to spatial patterns in oystercatcher feeding behaviour. Whilst at
the low intertidal and higher mid intertidal zones, characterised by comparatively thick shells, most empty shells had apparently
been stabbed, hammering was the dominant feeding behaviour at the lower mid intertidal zone, where shells were thinner. Preference
of hammering birds for smaller mussels was not ubiquitous. Sagittal shell shape was predominantly influenced by allometric
growth effects and had only minor effect on prey selection. All oystercatchers preferred less inflated mussels, with the degree
of shell inflation gradually increasing with higher intertidal elevation. Our results illustrate the importance of small-scale
patterns in prey ecophenotypes in determining the distribution and feeding dynamics of wading birds. 相似文献
10.
The diving behaviour of king penguins (Aptenodytes patagonicus) was studied on the Falkland Islands, where a small population (ca. 300 fledglings year–1) is located at the geographical limit of their breeding range. King penguins rearing newly hatched chicks were equipped with time-depth recorders before leaving for sea. In total, 20,175 dives >3 m were recorded from 12 birds during 15 foraging trips with a mean duration of 5.7±2.3 days. The majority of the trips was directed up to 500 km to the northeast of the breeding colony in slope waters of, and oceanic waters beyond, the Patagonian shelf. Mean time spent underwater accounted for 42±9% of the foraging trip. Mean dive depth achieved was 55±16 m; maximum dive depth recorded was 343 m. Mean dive duration was 159±25 s; maximum dive duration was 480 s. The mean vertical distance covered was 140±65 km trip–1; and on average birds covered 25 km day–1. Synchronous diving behaviour was observed in two birds for a period of about 24 h after leaving the colony. Dive depth correlated positively with: (1) light intensity, (2) dive duration and (3) vertical velocities, thus confirming previous findings obtained from conspecifics at other breeding sites and indicating comparable diving behaviour. However, separation of dives according to their profile—V-, U-, or W-shaped—revealed significant differences between certain dive parameters. For a given depth range, bottom time was longer and vertical velocities higher in W-dives than in U-dives. This, together with a higher number of W-dives at dawn and dusk, suggests that foraging is more effective during W-dives than U-dives, and during twilight. These findings imply that king penguins have to make more complex decisions, individually and socially, on the performance of the subsequent dive than previously thought.Communicated by O. Kinne, Oldendorf/Luhe 相似文献
11.
Although contemporary animal contest theory emphasises the importance of physical asymmetries in resolving disputes, such
asymmetries do not obviously settle fights in all groups. Territorial male butterflies, for example, compete via elaborate
non-contact aerial interactions in which success is determined by relative persistence. Prior research suggests that the resolution
of these contests is not clearly related to physical variables such as body size or energy reserves. However, given that the
contests involve elaborate aerial manoeuvres, one long-standing suggestion is that asymmetries in flight performance, and
thus flight morphology, may be important. We addressed this hypothesis via a manipulative investigation into the biophysical
correlates of contest success in the speckled wood butterfly, Pararge aegeria. This species possesses the ability for significant adaptive phenotypic plasticity in relevant flight morphological parameters.
We took advantage of this plasticity to rear 90 individuals of markedly varying flight morphologies, which we then pitted
against each other in a semi-controlled experimental fashion. Multiple logistic and lognormal analyses provided little evidence
for the relevance of morphological parameters, including relative flight musculature, wing loading and wing aspect ratio (wing
length relative to area), to the outcome and/or duration of experimental contests. Instead, we found a positive effect of
age upon contest success. Given that ability for high acceleration is strongly linked to variation in these morphological
parameters, our findings suggest that flight performance is not a strong determinant of resource-holding potential in this
notably territorial butterfly. 相似文献
12.
Summary. Scent signals are the main source of information transmission in carnivores, being particularly important for those with nocturnal
habits like the ringtail (Bassariscus astutus), a Procyonid widely distributed in Mexico. However, faecal marking behaviour of free-ranging ringtails has not been described
previously. The aims of this study were to describe the use of latrines in ringtails and to test if single faeces and latrines
have a marking function, based on the spatial characterisation of the defecation places and revisits to these sites. The study
was conducted at an urban reserve within Mexico City in 2003, where 80 defecation points were analysed. The results showed
that ringtails deposit faeces repeatedly in the same sites (latrines), which contain a variable number of faeces (between
2 and 19). The spatial distribution of latrines was not random, but a selection of substrates and zones that enhanced the
effectiveness as faecal marks was observed. Latrines were preferentially placed on objects above ground level and at road
margins. Latrines and single faeces that were in inconspicuous zones were generally deposited on conspicuous substrates and
latrines and single faeces that were in conspicuous zones were deposited more frequently on inconspicuous substrates. 相似文献
13.
Importance of jumbo squid Dosidicus gigas (Orbigny, 1835) in the pelagic ecosystem of the central Gulf of California 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
R. Rosas-Luis C.A. Salinas-Zavala V. Koch P. Del Monte Luna M.V. Morales-Zrate 《Ecological modelling》2008,218(1-2):149-161
The Humboldt squid is an important predator in the pelagic ecosystem of the central Gulf of California and the commercial catch of this species has increased over the past decade, probable due to a decrease of several top predators (sharks, large pelagic fish and the marine mammals) and the optimal feeding conditions in this area. Its high abundance and important position in the pelagic food web was quantified through two trophic models of the pelagic ecosystem of the central Gulf of California. Models represented conditions in 1980 and 2002, to document the decadal changes in ecosystem structure and function. The models were composed of 18 functional groups, including marine mammals, birds, fish, mollusks, crustaceans, and primary producers. Model results show direct negative effects on principal prey groups such as myctophids and pelagic red crab and positive effects on sharks, marine mammals and specifically sperm whales. It thus appears that the jumbo squid has an important role in the ecosystem and plays a central part in the overall energy flow as main food item for most top predators, and due to its predation of organisms on lower tropic levels. 相似文献
14.
Vladimír Remeš 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2005,58(3):326-332
Nest concealment varies strongly within populations of many species. Although some studies have revealed the beneficial effects of concealment in mitigating predation pressure on nests, other studies were unable to find similar effects. One potential reason for the mixed results is that parental behaviour may compensate for the effects of nest cover, and specifically designed experimental studies are needed to reveal this compensation. I studied the effects of concealment on the probability of nest survival in the blackcap (Sylvia atricapilla), by experimentally manipulating the degree of nest-foliage cover. There was a significant effect of the treatment depending on nest type and the phase of nesting. Whereas there was no effect of concealment on nest survival in natural nests, there was a positive effect in real nests baited with plasticine clutches (i.e. without parental activity). Parents probably behaviourally compensated for poor concealment in natural nests (nest guarding, defence). In line with this, there was no effect of concealment on nest survival during incubation, whereas there was probably a positive effect in the nestling phase. Parents spent more time on the nest during incubation (80%) than during the care of nestlings (40%) and, consequently, had more opportunities to compensate for poor cover. In general, we cannot use single measures of behaviours or states (nest concealment) as an indication of predation risk because of the capacity for compensation in other behaviours.Communicated by C. Brown 相似文献
15.
Seasonal variation in animal signalling behaviour has been well documented and has contributed much to our understanding of
male signals. In contrast, we know little about seasonal variation in female signals or signals produced jointly by males
and females, such as the vocal duets of birds. Here, we examine how singing behaviour changes in relation to time of year
and breeding stage in rufous-and-white wrens (Thryothorus rufalbus), neotropical songbirds where both males and females sing and where breeding partners coordinate songs to produce vocal duets.
We recorded a colour-marked population of birds over an extended time period encompassing multiple breeding stages. Across
all time frames and breeding stages, males sang at higher rates than females and male solos were more common than duets or
female solos. Males and females showed divergent seasonal patterns of singing. Females sang more often early in the year,
during the pre-breeding season, and female song tapered off as the breeding season progressed. Duetting followed a parallel
pattern, which resulted from females showing less duet responsiveness to their partner’s songs later in the year. Male independent
song rate peaked at the onset of the rainy season – a time when females become fertile – and males showed the highest level
of duet responsiveness during this period. Our results suggest that early in the year, duets appear to be cooperative displays,
functioning in joint territory defence and/or the coordination of breeding activities. When females are fertile, however,
increased duet responsiveness by males is consistent with mate or paternity guarding. 相似文献
16.
Nadine C. Chapman Benjamin P. Oldroyd William O. H. Hughes 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2007,61(8):1185-1194
Which task a social insect worker engages in is influenced by the worker’s age, genotype and the colony’s needs. In the honeybee,
Apis mellifera, genotype influences both the age a worker switches tasks and its propensity of engaging in specialist tasks, such as water
collecting, which only some workers will perform. In this study, we used colonies with natural levels of genetic diversity
and manipulated colony age demography to drastically increase the stimuli for the generalist tasks of foraging and nursing,
which all workers are thought to engage in at some point in their lives. We examined the representation of worker patrilines
engaged in nursing and foraging before and after the perturbation. The representation of patrilines among foragers and nurses
differed from that of their overall colony’s population. In the case of foraging, over- and underrepresentation of some patrilines
was not simply due to differences in rates of development among patrilines. We show that replacement foragers tend to be drawn
from patrilines that were overrepresented among foragers before the perturbation, suggesting that there is a genetic component
to the tendency to engage in foraging. In contrast, the representation of patrilines in replacement nurses differed from that
in the unperturbed nursing population. Our results show that there is a genetic influence on even the generalist tasks of
foraging and nursing, and that the way patrilines in genetically diverse colonies respond to increases in task stimuli depends
upon the task. The possible significance of this genetic influence on task allocation is discussed.
Electronic supplementary material Supplementary material is available in the online version of this article at doi: and is accessible to authorized users. 相似文献