共查询到17条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
K. A. Spencer J. H. Wimpenny K. L. Buchanan P. G. Lovell A. R. Goldsmith C. K. Catchpole 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2005,58(4):423-428
Developmental stress has recently been shown to have adverse effects upon adult male song structure in birds, which may well act as an honest signal of male quality to discriminating females. However, it still remains to be shown if females can discriminate between the songs of stressed and non-stressed males. Here we use a novel experimental design using an active choice paradigm to investigate preferences in captive female zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata). Nine females were exposed to ten pairs of songs by previously stressed and non-stressed birds that had learned their song from the same tutor. Song pairs differed significantly in terms of song complexity, with songs of stressed males exhibiting lower numbers of syllables and fewer different syllables in a phrase. Song rate and peak frequency did not differ between stressed and non-stressed males. Females showed a significant preference for non-stressed songs in terms of directed perching activity and time spent on perches. Our results therefore indicate that developmental stress affects not only the structure of male song, but that such structural differences are biologically relevant to female mate choice decisions. 相似文献
2.
Advertisement call duration indicates good genes for offspring feeding rate in gray tree frogs (Hyla versicolor) 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Indicator or ”good genes” models of sexual selection predict that mating preferences allow females to choose mates that are
genetically superior. Female gray tree frogs (Hyla versicolor) prefer male advertisement calls of long call duration, which can be indicators of enhanced offspring growth performance.
We tested the effects of father’s call duration and the presence of a caged predator (dragonfly naiad) on tadpole activity
and growth in a factorial experiment, controlling for maternal and environmental effects. The effect of food availability
(a repeated measure) on tadpole activity was also examined. Tadpoles responded to predator presence and to high food availability
by decreasing activity and feeding. Tadpoles exposed to a caged predator were smaller after 14 days than those exposed to
an empty cage, suggesting that spending less time feeding carries the cost of reduced growth. Offspring of males with long
versus short calls responded similarly to the presence of a predator. Nonetheless, offspring of long-calling males spent more
time feeding than did offspring of short-calling males, except when a predator was present but no food was available. Increased
time spent feeding may contribute to enhanced offspring growth and, therefore, to the indirect benefit that a female may realize
by selecting a mate with long calls. However, because the behavioral differences depended on the environment, and because
the fitness consequences of such behavioral differences should also vary with the environment, the benefit of mating with
a long-calling male may depend on the conditions encountered by the offspring.
Received: 15 February 2000 / Revised: 24 September 2000 / Accepted: 16 October 2000 相似文献
3.
T. R. Pope 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2000,48(4):253-267
Evaluation of evolutionary mechanisms proposed to promote cooperative behavior depends on the relative influence of the behavior
on the reproductive success of individuals, the reproductive success of the group in which they interact behaviorally, and
the degree of gene correlation among cooperators. The genetic relationship within cooperative coalitions of female red howler
monkeys was examined for three populations with different densities and growth rates. Patterns of gene correlation change
within coalitions is documented using data from the mitochondrial and nuclear genomes, and long-term census monitoring. Differences
in fecundity and infant survivorship within and between groups of unrelated (rˉ=0) and related (rˉ≥ 0.25) females are compared. Females that emigrate from their natal groups form coalitions with other migrant females. These
coalitions attempt to establish a territory and, once successful in producing offspring, exclude other females from feeding
resources. Females in these coalitions had different mtDNA haplotypes and a genetically estimated mean r of 0, supporting demographic data on emigration patterns indicating that these females rarely have the opportunity to form
coalitions with kin. Patterns of recruitment and rate of matriline development within social groups supported behavioral data
indicating that females actively attempt to promote their own matriline as breeders over that of other females, and that some
matrilines are more successful at this than others. Mean r among females was significantly higher in coalitions established as social groups for several generations (rˉ=0.44). In these groups, females all shared the same mtDNA haplotype, and mtDNA haplotype divergence was significantly higher
between than within groups. Females in coalitions with kin had significantly higher reproductive success than females in unrelated
coalitions in all populations. This difference was not a function of coalition size, number of males, socionomic sex ratio,
or primiparity, although anecdotal evidence suggests that allomothering may compensate for inept new mothers in related coalitions
more often than in unrelated ones. Differences in territory quality could not be ruled out as a potential causal factor in
the saturated populations, but were unlikely in the low-density, growing population. There were substantial differences among
long-established coalitions in overall reproductive output in all three populations, and this was significantly correlated
with the number of breeding females. Increase in coalition size was a function of both group age and the behavioral tolerance
among females. Regardless of the underlying reasons for the patterns observed, reproductive success clearly increases with
degree of gene correlation among females within cooperative coalitions, and coalitions that recruit more daughters produce
more offspring. The nature of the cooperative relationship among group females directly influences both of these outcomes.
This is associated with substantial genetic differentiation among social groups within populations, creating conditions in
which genetic tendencies towards cooperative behavior can become tightly associated with group reproductive success.
Received: 15 September 1999 / Revised: 27 April 2000 / Accepted: 27 May 2000 相似文献
4.
Fluctuating asymmetry (FA) in limb size in female decorated field crickets (Gryllodes sigillatus) was associated with a reduction in the size of the spermatophore and the amount of sperm transferred by males and an increase
in the time taken to transfer a spermatophore following introduction of a female. There was a weaker negative relationship
between limb asymmetry in males and sperm number but no significant relationship between asymmetry in either sex and spermatophylax
size. In line with a previous study, female size did not appear to influence spermatophore production or mating decisions
by males. The results imply that developmental instability affects both gamete production and mating decisions among males,
although the relationships between spermatophore size, sperm number and asymmetry in females are unlikely to be the result
of males perceiving differences in female FA.
Received: 24 July 1999 / Received in revised form: 22 November 1999 / Accepted: 31 December 1999 相似文献
5.
Testosterone and the allocation of reproductive effort in male house finches (Carpodacus mexicanus) 总被引:9,自引:1,他引:8
Testosterone has been proposed to serve as the mediator that controls the relative effort that an individual male bird will
devote to mating effort versus parental effort. Here, we demonstrate a testosterone-influenced trade-off between parental
and mating efforts in male house finches. Male house finches with experimentally elevated testosterone fed nestlings at a
significantly lower rate, but sang at a higher rate than males without manipulated testosterone levels. Females mated to testosterone-implanted
males fed nestlings at a significantly higher rate than females mated to males without testosterone implants, resulting in
similar feeding rates for both treated and untreated pairs. The effects of testosterone on male house finches, however, were
not as dramatic as the effects of testosterone observed in some other socially monogamous species of birds. Because extra-pair
copulations are uncommon in house finches and males provide substantial amounts of parental care, these more modest effects
may be due to differences in how the allocation of reproductive effort affects the costs and benefits of different reproductive
behaviors.
Received: 6 June 2000 / Accepted: 17 July 2000 相似文献
6.
Brood sex ratios, female harem status and resources for nestling provisioning in the great reed warbler (Acrocephalus arundinaceus) 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
H. Westerdahl Staffan Bensch Bengt Hansson Dennis Hasselquist Torbjörn von Schantz 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2000,47(5):312-318
The theory of parental investment and brood sex ratio manipulation predicts that parents should invest in the more costly
sex during conditions when resources are abundant. In the polygynous great reed warbler, Acrocephalus arundinaceus, females of primary harem status have more resources for nestling provisioning than secondary females, because polygynous
males predominantly assist the primary female whereas the secondary female has to feed her young alone. Sons weigh significantly
more than daughters, and are hence likely to be the more costly sex. In the present study, we measured the brood sex ratio
when the chicks were 9 days old, i.e. the fledging sex ratio. As expected from theory, we found that female great reed warblers
of primary status had a higher proportion of sons in their broods than females of lower (secondary) harem status. This pattern
is in accordance with the results from two other species of marsh-nesting polygynous birds, the oriental reed warbler, Acrocephalus orientalis, and the yellow-headed blackbird Xanthocephalus xanthocephalus. As in the oriental reed warbler, we found that great reed warbler males increased their share of parental care as the proportion
of sons in the brood increased. We did not find any difference in fitness of sons and daughters raised in primary and secondary
nests. The occurrence of adaptive sex ratio manipulations in birds has been questioned, and it is therefore important that
three studies of polygynous bird species, including our own, have demonstrated the same pattern of a male-biased offspring
sex ratio in primary compared with secondary nests.
Received: 1 June 1999 / Received in revised form: 10 January 2000 / Accepted: 12 February 2000 相似文献
7.
Dominique Berteaux Joël Bêty Eddie Rengifo Jean-Marie Bergeron 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1999,45(3-4):283-291
Multiple paternity in single litters conceived in the wild was recently demonstrated in meadow voles (Microtuspennsylvanicus). In this study, we used an experimental approach (males tethered and females allowed to mate freely with one or several males) to investigate the role of female meadow voles in multiple paternity. We found that among 29 (of 39) females that copulated during our experiment, 79.3% chose to mate with more than one male. Female behavior in meadow voles thus clearly promotes multiple paternity and their role is an active one. Some of the hypotheses explaining promiscuity in meadow voles should be reconsidered in light of this result. We do not know the primary determinant of female mate choice, but male body mass played a secondary role in driving female preferences. The partial dependence between male body mass and female choice, coupled with the active role played by females, indicates that intersexual selection has the potential for reinforcing the effects of intrasexual selection (male-male dominance relationships) in this species. Finally, we demonstrate that the time period over which tests are conducted is an important part of the design of experiments aimed at understanding the role of females in multiple paternity. Received: 14 April 1998 / Accepted after revision: 12 September 1998 相似文献
8.
Summary. This study reports on the impact of insecticidal resistance on the diel periodicity of the calling behaviour and pheromone
production of different-aged virgin females of the obliquebanded leafroller (OBL), Choristoneura rosaceana. While both resistant (R) and susceptible (S) females initiated calling on the first night following emergence, the periodicity
of the calling behaviour, as determined by the mean onset time of calling (MOTC) and the mean time spent calling (MTSC) over
the first six nights of calling, differed between the two strains. R females started calling significantly later in the night.
However, as the MOTC of R females advanced with age but did not do so in S individuals, the difference between strains was
more pronounced in younger than older females. Furthermore, R females spent less time calling than S individuals. However,
the MTSC increased as a function of age in both R and S females, so the difference between strains remained fairly constant
for each night of calling. The major component of OBL sex pheromone, the Z11-14: Ac, determined at peak calling activity,
significantly declined with female age. Overall, pheromone production was lower in R females than in S females, with the difference
being more pronounced in younger than in older individuals. Thus, resistant females may have a lower mating success. The mating
success of both R and S strain males did not vary with the number of previous matings acquired. With regard to males, although
there was a significant decline in spermatophore size with successive matings, there was no significant difference between
strains. However, R males are smaller and may be disadvantaged through female choice and/or may respond differently to pheromone
source compared with S individuals. If the reproductive success of both sexes is affected, this may have a profound influence
on the dynamics of insecticidal resistance in the presence or absence of selection in OBL populations.
Received 4 July 2001; accepted 19 October 2001. 相似文献
9.
We experimentally studied the relative importance of plumage, dominance status, and courtship behavior in determining male pairing success in the northern pintail Anas acuta and assessed whether these traits function in female choice, male-male competition or both. In an experiment (experiment IA) that eliminated the confounding effects of male-male competition and social courtship, females chose males with pure white breasts and colorful scapular feathers. When the same group of birds were free to interact (experiment 1B), male behavior was more important: females chose males that courted them intensely and were attentive to them, although preferred males again had whiter breasts and more colorful scapulars. In a second experiment (experiment 2), testing the effect of age on pairing success, females showed a significant preference for 2-year-old males over yearlings: 2-year-old males courted more and were more attentive to the female than yearlings; they were also more colorful than yearlings in a number of plumage measurements. Although males (in both experiments 1B and 2) were aggressive to one another while courting the female and dominant males were sometimes able to exclude subordinates from social courtship, contrary to expectation, we found no relationship between initial dominance rank and pairing success or dominance rank and age. In addition, dominance was not correlated with any of the morphological traits measured. Once chosen, however, subordinate males typically initiated fights with the higher-ranked male(s) and quickly achieved dominance. These results suggest that (1) females choose males based on a suite of morphological and behavioral characteristics, (2) male dominance relationships do not constrain active female choice, (3) a male's position in a dominance hierarchy is largely a result rather than a cause of female choice, and (4) female choice plays a more significant role than male-male competition in the evolution of several secondary sexual traits in male northern pintails. 相似文献
10.
Leg ornamentation and the efficacy of courtship display in four species of wolf spider (Araneae: Lycosidae) 总被引:1,自引:1,他引:0
This study used both correlative and experimental video playback methods to test the hypothesis that the secondary sexual
traits of male wolf spiders act to increase the efficacy of visual courtship displays. Direct observations of courtship of
several lycosid genera and a review of the literature revealed a significant association between ornamentation and visual
courtship displays. This suggests that the ornamentation may be playing the role of amplifier for a visual display. To test
this hypothesis, male courtship behaviors of four Schizocosa species were experimentally manipulated using video-imaging techniques. Females of species with non-visually displaying,
non-ornamented males (Schizocosa duplex and S. uetzi) did not increase in frequency of receptivity when tufts were added to conspecific males. In a species with a visual display
and foreleg pigmentation (S. stridulans), the addition of foreleg tufts increased female receptivity. In a tufted species (S. crassipes), females tended to decrease their receptivity when male ornamentation was completely removed. In visually displaying species,
ornamentation acts to increase female receptivity, supporting its role as an amplifier of a visual display.
Received: 29 December 1997 / Received in revised form: 23 October 1999 / Accepted: 13 December 1999 相似文献
11.
Divorce and extrapair mating in female black-capped chickadees (Parus atricapillus): separate strategies with a common target 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
S. M. Ramsay K. A. Otter D. J. Mennill L. M. Ratcliffe P. T. Boag 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2000,49(1):18-23
Patterns of divorce and extrapair mating can provide insights into the targets of female choice in free-living birds. In resident,
site-faithful species with continuous partnerships, the better options and the incompatibility hypotheses provide the most
likely explanations for divorce. Extrapair mating can be explained by a number of hypotheses often making similar predictions.
For example, the good genes and future partnerships hypo- theses predict similar patterns if males with good genes also make
the best future partners. By considering both divorce and extrapair mating, it may be possible to distinguish between these
comparable hypotheses. We examined natural patterns of divorce and extrapair mating in a long-term study of black-capped chickadees
(Parus atricapillus). Out of 144 partnerships over 8 years, we observed 11 divorces and 38 faithful pairs between seasons. Females usually divorced
between their first and second breeding seasons for males of higher social rank than their previous partners, had similar
reproductive success prior to divorce as females who retained their previous partners, and did not divorce on the basis of
previous reproductive success. These results confirm earlier experimental evidence that females divorce for better options.
Females who divorced were significantly more likely to have had mixed-paternity broods prior to divorce than females who stayed
with their previous partners. There was no evidence that females divorced in favour of previous extrapair partners. These
results support the good genes hypothesis for extrapair mating, suggesting that female chickadees use divorce and multiple
mating as separate strategies sharing a common target.
Received: 4 February 2000 / Revised: 20 July 2000 / Accepted: 4 September 2000 相似文献
12.
Food distribution is hypothesized to be important in determining the nature of female relationships within social groups of
primates. When food limits female reproductive success, spatially clumped foods are expected to produce strong, linear dominance
hierarchies within groups, whereas more spatially dispersed foods are expected to produce weaker or non-existent dominance
hierarchies. The association between food distribution and competitive relationships presumably occurs because clumped foods
are usurpable but dispersed foods are not. We examined the spatial distribution of food patches (trees) and patch size relative
to feeding behavior and agonistic interactions in vervets and patas monkeys, two closely related and sympatric species that
nonetheless differ in the strength of the female dominance hierarchy. Food patches of both patas monkeys and vervets were
small in size and randomly distributed in Acacia drepanolobium habitat. In contrast, in A. xanthophloea woodland, the habitat type that was exclusively used by vervets, food patches were larger and more spatially clumped. These
similarities and differences between and within species were correlated with similarities and differences in the strength
and linearity of their dominance hierarchies. Patas monkeys and vervets in A. drepanolobium habitat had dominance hierarchies that were weakly defined because there were relatively few agonistic interactions between
females. By contrast, in A. xanthophloea habitat, vervets had a stronger, linear dominance hierarchy characterized by a higher rate of agonistic interactions over
food. The covariation of agonistic interactions with patch size is discussed in relation to depletion time, another characteristic
that may covary with food distribution, and resource renewal rate, an important determinant of agonistic interactions in insectivorous
birds, fishes, insects, and mammals.
Received: 18 February 2000 / Revised: 5 September 2000 / Accepted: 26 September 2000 相似文献
13.
Masayo Soma Mariko Hiraiwa-Hasegawa Kazuo Okanoya 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2009,63(3):363-370
Birdsong differs from other sexual traits in that the acquisition process involves learning. Especially in close-ended learning
species like the Bengalese finch, conditions experienced during the critical song-learning period can have a profound influence
on song quality. Therefore, to understand song evolution from a life-history perspective, we investigated early ontogenetic
effects on song quality. In particular, we focused on maternal effects and sibling competition. In asynchronously hatching
bird species, the age hierarchy among nestlings affects physical development due to competition for food; mothers may influence
this competition by adjusting their investment in each egg according to its sequence in the laying order. To independently
assess these effects, chicks of the Bengalese finch were cross-fostered so that the age hierarchies formed in fostered broods
were independent of the laying order. Our results indicate that song quality partially reflects early ontogenetic conditions,
whereas song duration and note-type repertoire were independent of either laying order or age hierarchy. The syntactical complexity
of note order declined over the laying sequence. This finding suggests that the song learning ability is influenced by within-clutch
variation in maternal investment toward eggs. Considering that song syntactical complexity is subject to female preference
in the Bengalese finch, it is likely that maternal resource allocation strategies play a role in song evolution. 相似文献
14.
C. Cordero 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2000,48(6):458-462
The differential costs of mating paid by males and females influence the nature and strength of sexual selection. In butterflies,
males invest a relatively large amount of time and resources in each mating, but male survival costs of mating have not been
demonstrated. I present the results of experiments designed to measure the effect of different aspects of mating on male longevity
in the polygynous butterfly Callophrys xami. In experiment 1, I compared the longevity of pairs of males that produced similar amounts of spermatophore, but that mated
at different rates, a different numbers of times, and that produced spermatophores at different rates, and found that the
longevity of ”low-mating-rate” males was not different from that of ”high-mating-rate” males. In experiment 2, the longevity
of virgin males was not significantly different from that of multiply mated males. In experiment 3, I used resource-limited
males resulting from experimental food limitation of last-instar larvae; resource-limited virgin males lived significantly
more days than resource-limited multiply mated males. Since ecological costs of mating (e.g., disease transmission, predation
risk) were excluded in the experiment, diminished male longevity was a product of physiological costs of sexual interactions.
These results suggest that the cost of ejaculate production is an important cause of longevity reduction when there are resource
limitations; however, the role of other possible physiological costs of mating in longevity reduction is still unknown.
Received: 21 March 2000 / Accepted: 26 August 2000 相似文献
15.
BriAnne Addison Alexander S. Kitaysky J. Mark Hipfner 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2008,63(1):135-141
Sex allocation theory posits that mothers should preferentially invest in sons when environmental conditions are favorable
for breeding, their mates are of high quality, or they are in good body condition. We tested these three hypotheses in rhinoceros
auklets (Cerorhinca monocerata), monomorphic seabirds that lay a single-egg clutch, in 2 years that differed in environmental conditions for breeding. Results
supported the environment and mate quality hypotheses, but these effects were interactive: offspring sex was independent of
paternal traits in the poor year for breeding, while females mated to larger and more ornamented males reared more sons in
the better year. Conversely, offspring sex was unrelated to female condition, as indexed by hatching date. We propose that
good rearing conditions enable females to rear sons possessing the desirable phenotypic attributes of their mates. Results
also supported two critical assumptions of sex allocation theory: (1) dimorphism in offspring condition at independence: daughters
fledged with higher baseline levels of corticosterone than sons and (2) differential costs of rearing sons versus daughters:
mothers rearing sons when environmental conditions were poor completed parental care in poorer condition than mothers rearing
daughters in the same year and mothers rearing either sex when conditions were better. These novel results may help to explain
the disparate results of previous studies of avian sex allocation. 相似文献
16.
Two species of closely related wood cricket, Gryllus fultoni (Orthoptera: Gryllidae) and Gryllus vernalis, occur together in some parts of the eastern United States and have a similar calling song structure, consisting of three-pulse chirps. A previous study revealed that chirp rate in G. fultoni was highest (greatest difference vis-à-vis chirp rate in G. vernalis) in sympatric populations, intermediate in near allopatric populations that were located close to the sympatric zone, and lowest in allopatric populations. A similar trend was observed in pulse rate, but the mean values of this trait showed much more convergence than chirp rate at the low end of the range of calling temperatures. In this study, we investigated the song discrimination of females from sympatric and allopatric populations of G. fultoni at about 23°C, which is near the middle of the normal range of calling temperatures. We used both single-stimulus and two-stimulus playback experiments to learn if geographical differences in song preferences paralleled those in calling songs. Stimuli presented were representative of calling songs in three classes of G. fultoni populations (sympatric, near allopatric, and far allopatric), a calling song of G. vernalis, and three calling songs with parameter values that were intermediate with respect to those of the songs of far allopatric G. fultoni and G. vernalis. In the single-stimulus playbacks, females of all G. fultoni populations responded poorly if at all to the heterospecific stimulus. Females of sympatric and near allopatric populations responded poorly to all intermediate stimuli, but females of far allopatric populations frequently responded to these sounds. In the two-stimulus playbacks, females of sympatric and near allopatric populations generally discriminated against intermediate and heterospecific stimuli. However, females of far allopatric populations often did not discriminate against intermediate stimuli, whose characteristics resembled the calling songs of G. vernalis. The divergent pattern of female phonotactic discrimination between sympatric and far allopatric populations was thus generally congruent with the pattern of divergence in chirp and pulse rates and would be expected to significantly reduce heterospecific mating in sympatry. These geographical patterns of female song discrimination and male calling songs conform to a commonly used definition of reproductive character displacement. 相似文献
17.
Fisher’s 1930 theory of sex allocation predicts a population-wide 1:1 ratio of parental investment. We tested this prediction in the European beewolf, a sphecid wasp that hunts for honeybees as larval food. Because the method to quantify parental investment is of crucial importance, we compared the suitability of several different investment measures. Female/male cost ratios were determined from a sample and the total investment in sons and daughters was calculated. In addition, the actual number of prey items for sons and daughters was directly determined by excavating nests and counting the cuticle remains of the prey. Though mortality was high (70%), it had only a weak effect on the estimate of the investment ratio. Based on commonly used measures like fresh and dry weight of emerged adults, the investment ratio did not deviate from Fisher’s prediction of equal investment. However, progeny weight considerably underestimates investment in males and investment in large progeny. Measures that reflect the allocation of resources more directly (amount of provisions, brood cell volume) revealed a significant male bias and thus contradicted Fisher’s theory. Three kinds of explanation are discussed. First, non-adaptive explanations are unlikely. Second, from the spectrum of alternative adaptive theories, only models that assume a non-linear relationship between amount of investment and progeny fitness seem to be relevant for the study species. Third, though the number of prey in a brood cell seems to be a rather good measure of parental investment in European beewolves, some problems in measuring parental investment remain. These problems are of broad significance. Received: 17 June 1999 / Received in revised form: 6 July 1999 / Accepted: 11 July 1999 相似文献