共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
The study of location specification in recruitment communication by bees has focused on two dimensions: direction and distance from the nest. Yet the third dimension, height above ground, may be significant in the tall and dense forest habitats of stingless bees. Foragers of the stingless bee Scaptotrigona postica recruit to a specific three-dimensional location by laying a scent trail. Stingless bees in the genus Melipona are thought to have a more sophisticated recruitment system that communicates distance through sounds inside the nest and direction through pointing zig-zag flights outside the nest. However, prior research on Melipona has not examined height communication or even established that foragers can recruit newcomers to a specific location. We used identical paired feeders to investigate recruitment to food in M panamica on Barro Colorado Island, Panama. We trained foragers from an observation hive to one feeder and monitored both feeders for the subsequent arrival of newcomers. We changed the relative positions of the feeders to test for correct direction, distance, and canopy-level communication. A 40-m canopy tower located inside the forest enabled us to examine canopy-level communication. We found that M. panamica foragers can recruit to a specific (1) direction, (2) distance, and (3) canopy level. To test the possibility that foragers accomplish this by means of a scent trail, we placed the colony on one shore of a small cove and trained bees over 116 m of open water to a feeder located on the opposite shore. We also placed a second feeder on this shore, equidistant from the colony but 20 m from the first feeder. Significantly more newcomers consistently arrived at the feeder visited by the foragers. Thus foragers evidently do not need a scent trail to communicate direction. Inside the nest, a forager produces pulsed sounds while visibly vibrating her wings after returning from a good food source. She is attended by other bees who cluster and hold their antennae around her, following her as she rapidly spins clockwise and counterclockwise. Locational information may be encoded in this behavior. However, foragers may also directly lead newcomers to the food source. Further experiments are planned to test for such piloting and other communication mechanisms. 相似文献
2.
James C. Nieh 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1998,43(1):47-58
Melipona panamica foragers can deposit a scent beacon that influences the orientation of foragers near a food source. In misdirection experiments,
newcomers (bees from the same colony as trained foragers) consistently preferred the feeder at which trained foragers had
fed (training feeder) over an identical feeder at which bees had never fed (control feeder) even when the training feeder
was placed at a site where experienced foragers had never foraged. Through similar misdirection experiments, the effective
radius of the scent beacon was determined to be greater than 6 and less than 12 m. Foragers may deposit this beacon during
a sequence of departure behaviors performed at the feeder. Prior to leaving the feeder with a load of sugar solution, bees
tended to perform the following sequence of behaviors: (1) spinning, (2) grooming, (3) abdomen dragging, (4) excreting anal
droplets, and (5) producing sounds, although not all behaviors were performed prior to each departure or at all sucrose concentrations
(0.5–2.5 m). As sucrose concentration increased, the number of newcomers significantly increased, and the number of experienced foragers
producing sounds and spinning on the feeder increased. The exact source of the scent beacon remains a mystery. However, three
important sources have been excluded. When choosing between identical paired feeders, foragers were not attracted to the feeders
with (1) anal droplets, (2) extracts of sucrose solution at which foragers had fed, or (3) mandibular gland extracts. They
were indifferent to the first two preparations and exhibited only typical alarm behavior towards the mandibular gland (MG)
extract: they oriented towards the feeder with MG extract but consistently landed on the feeder with no MG extract. Other
authors have suggested that Melipona foragers deposit anal droplets to attract recruits, however the frequency of anal droplet production and the mass of anal
droplets produced by M. panamica foragers are negatively correlated with sucrose concentration. Thus the scent beacon is evidently not deposited with anal
droplets, infused into the feeder solution, or produced by mandibular glands.
Received: 2 September 1997 / Accepted after revision: 30 January 1998 相似文献
3.
Potential mechanisms for the communication of height and distance by a stingless bee, Melipona panamica 总被引:1,自引:1,他引:0
This study investigates the recruitment communication mechanisms of a stingless bee, Melipona panamica, whose foragers can evidently communicate the three-dimensional location of a good food source. To determine if the bees
communicate location information inside or outside the nest, we conducted removal experiments by training marked foragers
to one of two identical feeders and then separating these experienced foragers from potential recruits as they left the nest.
The feeders were positioned to test the communication of each dimension. The results show that recruits do not simply follow
experienced foragers to the food source. Height and distance are communicated within the nest, while direction is communicated
outside the nest. We then examined the pulsed sounds produced by recruiting foragers. While unloading food, recruiting foragers
produced several short pulses and one or more very long pulses. On average, the longest unloading pulse per performance was
31–50% longer (P ≤ 0.018) for bees foraging on the forest floor than for bees foraging at the top of the forest canopy (40 m high). While
dancing, recruiting foragers produced sound pulses whose duration was positively correlated with the distance to the food
source (P < 0.001). Dancing recruiters also produced several short sound pulses followed by one or more long pulses. The longest dance
pulse per performance was 291 ± 194 ms for a feeder 25 m from the nest and 1858 ± 923 ms for a feeder 360 m away from the
nest. The mechanism of directional communication remains a mystery. However, the direction removal experiment demonstrates
that newcomers cannot use forager-deposited scent marks for long-distance orientation (>100 m from the nest).
Received: 25 September 1997 / Accepted after revision: 31 May 1998 相似文献
4.
Queen control of egg fertilization in the honey bee 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
The study investigated the precision with which honey bee queens can control the fertilization of the eggs they lay. Because
males and workers are reared in different-sized cells, the honey bee is one of the few Hymenoptera in which it is possible
for the experimenter to know which type of egg a queen “intends” to lay. Eggs were collected from both worker and drone (male)
cells from four honey bee colonies. Ploidy of the embryo was determined using polymorphic DNA microsatellites. All 169 eggs
taken from worker cells were heterozygous at at least one microsatellite locus showing that the egg was fertilized. All 129
eggs taken from drone cells gave a single band at the B124 locus, strongly suggesting haploidy. These data show that honey
bee queens have great, and quite possibly complete, ability to control the fertilization of the eggs they lay. Data from the
literature suggest that in two species of parasitoid Hymenoptera (Copidosoma floridanum, Colpoclypeus florus) females have great, but not complete, ability to control fertilization.
Received: 23 December 1997 / Accepted after revision: 17 May 1998 相似文献
5.
In an experimental set-up, a colony of the stingless bee Melipona fasciata demonstrated its ability to choose the better of two nectar sources. This colony pattern was a result of the following individual
behavioural decisions: continue foraging, abandon the feeder, restart foraging and initiate foraging. Only very rarely did
individuals switch from one feeder to the other. With the first combination of a rich (2.7 M) and a poor (0.8 M) feeder M. fasciata behaved differently from Apis mellifera. Recruitment occurred to both feeders and the poor feeder was not abandoned completely. When the poor feeder was set to 0.4 M,
M. fasciata abandoned the poor feeder rapidly and allocated more foragers to the rich feeder. These patterns were similar to those reported
for A. mellifera with the first combination of feeders. Over a sequence of 4 days, experienced bees increasingly determined the colony patterns,
and the major function of communication between workers became the reactivation of experienced foragers. The foragers modulated
their behaviour not only according to the profitability of the feeder, but also according to previous experience with profitability
switches. Thus, experience and communication together regulated colony foraging behaviour. These findings and the results of studies with honeybees suggest
that M. fasciata and honeybees use similar decision-making mechanisms and only partly different tools.
Received: 21 December 1998 / Accepted: 5 January 1999 相似文献
6.
7.
8.
Jürgen Gadau Pia J. Gertsch Jürgen Heinze Pekka Pamilo Bert Hölldobler 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1998,44(1):23-33
Multilocus DNA fingerprinting and microsatellite analysis were used to determine the number of queens and their mating frequencies
in colonies of the carpenter ant, Camponotus ligniperdus (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Only 1 of 61 analyzed queens was found to be double-mated and the population-wide effective mating
frequency was therefore 1.02. In the studied population, 8 of 21 mature field colonies (38%) contained worker, male, or virgin
queen genotypes which were not compatible with presumed monogyny and therefore suggested oligogyny, i.e., the cooccurrence
of several mutually intolerant queens within one colony. Estimated queen numbers in oligogynous colonies ranged between two
and five. According to the results of the genetic analysis, most of the queens coexisting in oligogynous colonies were not
closely related. Pleometrosis is very rare and queenless colonies adopt mated queens both in the laboratory and field. Therefore,
the most plausible explanation for the origin of oligogynous colonies in C. ligniperdus is the adoption of unrelated queens by orphaned mature colonies. The coexistence of unrelated, but mutually intolerant queens
in C. ligniperdus colonies demonstrates that oligogyny should be considered as a phenomenon distinct from polygyny.
Received: 18 December 1997 / Accepted after revision: 20 June 1998 相似文献
9.
Scott Nunes Peter A. Zugger Anne L. Engh Kurt O. Reinhart Kay E. Holekamp 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,40(3):199-207
We examined the effects of food provisioning on the natal dispersal behavior of Belding's ground squirrels (Spermophilus beldingi). We provided extra food to adult and yearling females in their maternal territories during pregnancy and lactation, and
to offspring of these females in their natal areas for 6 weeks after weaning. We used unprovisioned young of unprovisioned
mothers as controls. Provisioning influenced the probability of dispersal from the natal area by female but not male S. beldingi. All surviving male S.␣beldingi dispersed by 55 weeks of age, regardless of whether they and their mothers received extra food. By contrast, we observed
a significant trend, beginning 3 weeks after weaning and continuing through the yearling year, for a greater proportion of
provisioned than control female S. beldingi to emigrate from the natal area. Competition for food did not appear to influence natal dispersal of females. However, overall
population density, density of females weaning litters, and rates of aggression and vigilance among these females, were higher
in provisioned than control areas, suggesting that competition for non-food resources was unusually intense in provisioned
areas. We propose that juvenile female, but not juvenile male, S. beldingi may emigrate from the natal site to increase access to areas with low densities of conspecifics. Together with findings of
earlier workers, our results suggest that spatial and temporal distributions of environmental resources are important influences
on the dispersal behavior of female ground squirrels.
Received: 28 February 1996 / Accepted after revision: 16 October 1996 相似文献
10.
Coalitionary mate guarding by male chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda 总被引:13,自引:0,他引:13
David P. Watts 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1998,44(1):43-55
Cooperative mate guarding by males is unusual in mammals and birds, largely because fertilizations are non-shareable. Chimpanzees
live in fission-fusion communities that have cores of philopatric males who cooperate in inter-group aggression and in defending
access to the females in their community. Male contest mating competition is restrained within communities, but single high-ranking
males sometimes try to mate guard estrous females. Data from an unusually large chimpanzee commmunity at Ngogo, Kibale National
Park, Uganda, that contains more males than any previously studied community show new variation in chimpanzee mate-guarding
behavior. Contrary to expectation given the large number of males, mate guarding was as common as, or more common than, at
other sites, and males other than the alpha male guarded more often. More strikingly, pairs or trios of top-ranking males
sometimes engaged in cooperative aggression to prevent estrous females from mating with other males, but tolerated each other's
mating activities. Both single males and coalitions mostly guarded periovulatory females. Mate-guarding coalitions were previously
unknown in chimpanzees. Coalitions occurred in large mating parties, seemingly because these often contained too many males
for single males to maintain exclusive access to estrous females. Coalition members gained higher shares of copulations than
they could have expected from solo mate guarding, and suffered lower per capita costs of guarding (as inferred from aggression
rates). Two males who most often participated in coalitions formed two-male coalitions at about the point where the number
of males present made it unlikely that either could get 50% or more of total copulations on his own, and formed trios when
this value dropped below 33%. Kin selection could be a factor in cooperation among male chimpanzees, but coalition members
were not necessarily close relatives and the apparent structure of payoffs fit that of mutualism. Furthermore, reliance of
male chimpanzees on support from allies to maintain high rank could have led to trading of mating exclusivity for support
against mating competitors.
Received: 28 May 1997 / Accepted after revision: 16 May 1998 相似文献
11.
Food access, brood size and filial cannibalism in the fantail darter, Etheostoma flabellare 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
We compared the occurrence of filial cannibalism in fed and starved male fantail darters (Etheostoma flabellare). All males in the experiment consumed eggs, and 56% ate all of their eggs. A male's initial body condition did not explain
the number of eggs that he ate. Neither did non-fed males eat more eggs than fed males. Fed males were able to maintain better
body condition during the experiment, but the change in body condition also depended on the number of eggs eaten. Thus, males
who ate more eggs were able to maintain better body condition.The most important determinant of whether or not a male ate
all of his eggs was his initial egg number. Males with small egg masses ate all of their eggs whereas males with large egg
masses were only partial cannibals. There was, however, no difference in the total number of eggs eaten by total and partial
cannibals. We conclude that eggs are only partially eaten for energetic reasons. We also suggest that small egg masses are
completely consumed because the costs of caring for a small egg mass may exceed the expected reproductive benefits of a small
egg mass.
Received: 26 January 1996 / Accepted after revision: 2 November 1996 相似文献
12.
B. Sklepkovych 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,40(5):287-296
Foraging competition in Siberian jay groups was examined in relation to dominance and kinship to determine whether juvenile
offspring, by associating with adults, gained in food acquisition relative to juvenile immigrants. Members of the adult pair
were dominant over juvenile cohort members and males were dominant to females, although an inter-sexual hierarchy, with male
juveniles occasionally overlapping adult females, was suggested. Few competitive asymmetries were found between adults and
retained offspring or adults and immigrant juveniles when they were competing for food together, but in kin and non-kin foraging
groups, respectively. Male offspring visited the bait site more frequently than adult males, and female immigrants spent less
time at the bait site than adult females. Under these circumstances, hoarding activities may limit the ability of alpha members
to control resources. In mixed groups containing both juvenile offspring and juvenile immigrants, no difference was found
in the number of visits made to the bait site, although load sizes and foraging rates were lower for immigrant birds. Retained
juveniles obtained greater load sizes and foraging rates when associating with adults. The social dominance of parents suggests
that they control juvenile foraging. Although offspring benefit in the presence of adults, adults may incur a cost to their
restraint by spending more time at the bait site when competing with immigrants. These results extend conclusions from previous
work describing the role of selective tolerance by adults which relaxes competition with retained offspring in Siberian jay
winter groups. The present findings suggest that offspring benefit in both immediate and future energy gains, which may have
a direct influence on survival.
Received: 18 September 1996 / Accepted after revision: 26 January 1997 相似文献
13.
Allison J. Abell 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,41(4):217-226
The association between spatial proximity and paternity was studied in a population of the striped plateau lizard, Sceloporus virgatus. The relationship between estimated mating success and male phenotypic traits was examined for a sample of 55 males. DNA
samples were obtained from 13 female-offspring families. The males with the closest spatial proximity to each female were
tested as possible sires within each family. Fingerprinting with two multilocus hypervariable minisatellite probes revealed
a strong correspondence between male-female spatial proximity and actual paternity. Paternity could be assigned for 72 of
the 100 hatchlings. Most hatchlings with identifiable sires were attributed to a male with the highest category of spatial
proximity to the mother. However, there was a low to moderate level of multiple paternity within clutches, and for some clutches
probable sires could not be identified even though the most likely behavioural candidates were tested. Thus, nonterritorial
males or other males lacking strong social and spatial relationships with females may achieve some degree of reproductive
success. Analysis of mating success revealed that male success increased with body size, up to a point beyond which larger
size conferred no advantage.
Received: 7 January 1997 / Accepted after revision: 16 June 1997 相似文献
14.
Arjan M. Boonman Martijn Boonman Frank Bretschneider Wim A. van de Grind 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1998,44(2):99-107
Daubenton's bat, a trawling vespertilionid bat species, hunts for insects that fly close to, or rest on, the water surface.
During summer, many ponds at which Daubenton's bats hunt become gradually covered with duckweed. The purpose of this study
was to investigate the effects of duckweed cover on the hunting behaviour of Daubenton's bats and on the ultrasound-reflecting
properties of the water surface. Our study revealed the following. (1) Daubenton's bat avoids water surfaces covered with
duckweed. (2) Prey abundance was related to the number of foraging Daubenton's bats but was independent of duckweed cover.
(3) When mealworms were presented among standardized amounts of duckweed to naturally foraging Daubenton's bats, they caught
significantly less mealworms when the duckweed cover was increased. (4) Measurements with ultrasonic signals show that a water
surface covered with duckweed returns a much stronger background echo at small angles (i.e. parallel to the water surface)
compared to an uncovered water surface. It seems likely that a cover of duckweed on the water surface interferes with prey
detection by masking the echoes returning from prey. (5) It was relatively difficult for the bats to discriminate small patches
of duckweed from mealworms. The proposed discrimination mechanism for this trawling bat species suggests that single duckweed
patches can also be mistaken for natural prey by Daubenton's bats.
Received: 4 January 1998 / Accepted after revision: 19 July 1998 相似文献
15.
It is unclear whether stingless bees in the genus Melipona (Hymenoptera, Apidae, Meliponini) can reliably encode the distance to a food source through recruitment sounds produced inside the nest, in part because the sound features correlated with distance also vary with food quality. We therefore trained marked foragers of two species, Melipona mandacaia and M. bicolor, to feeders at different distances and to different sucrose concentrations at the same distance. In both species, foragers successfully recruited to a rich 2.5-m food source and produced pulsed recruitment sounds in which pulse duration was significantly and positively correlated with distance to the rich food source. When returning from poorer food sources (0.6–1.5 m), foragers of both species decreased sound production, producing shorter sound pulses and longer sound interpulses than they did for 2.5 m food located at the same distance. Thus the temporal structure of M. mandacaia and M. bicolor recruitment sounds varies with distance and food quality. However, nestmates were not recruited by performances for poorer food sources (0.6–1.5 m), whose sucrose concentration was sufficiently low to affect recruitment sounds. Surprisingly, the interphase (the time between behavioral phases that communicate location) also increases with decreasing food quality in the closely related honeybees (Apis), suggesting a potential homology in the effect of food quality on the recruitment systems of Apis and Melipona. We explore the evolutionary implications of these similarities.Communicated by M. Giurfa 相似文献
16.
Social monogamy and extra-pair fertilization in an Australian lizard, Tiliqua rugosa 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
C. Michael Bull Steven J. B. Cooper Ben C. Baghurst 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1998,44(1):63-72
This study investigates social monogamy in the Australian sleepy lizard, Tiliqua rugosa. At a 70-ha site near Mount Mary, South Australia, we radio tracked 55 adult female and 39 adult male lizards during their
spring activity periods. Each lizard was observed in 1–5 years. Females were observed with a single male partner on an average
of 10.8 days per year, although in 17.3% of cases, females were observed on 2 or fewer days with a male. The most intense
pairing period each year was 15 September–15 November when females were with male partners on an average of 36% of observation
days. Partnerships lasted an average of 43.3 days each year. After mating in early November, the pairs separated. Observations
of females pairing with other males were rare. Most males (82%) were also consistently monogamous, although 7 were observed
pairing with 2 females within one season. To investigate paternity, we allowed 21 gravid females to give birth to 42 offspring
in the laboratory. We determined genotypes at five polymorphic microsatellite DNA loci for the females, their male partners
and their offspring. Four litters (19%) and 6 of the offspring from those litters (14.3%) showed evidence of extra-pair fertilization
(EPF). Although the sample sizes are small, females of polygynous males were more likely to experience EPF.
Received: 22 February 1998 / Accepted after revision: 23 May 1998 相似文献
17.
Adjustment of parental effort in the puffin; the roles of adult body condition and chick size 总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6
K. E. Erikstad M. Asheim P. Fauchald L. Dahlhaug Torkild Tveraa P. Dahlhaug 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,40(2):95-100
We examined the adjustment of parental effort of puffins by switching 20-day-old chicks randomly between parents of known
body condition. Among unmanipulated birds mass gain (5–20 days) and mass of 20-day-old chicks was positively correlated with
the body condition of parents at day 6. During the first 5 days after chick switching 28% (n = 55) of the parents deserted their foster chick. Parents which deserted their foster chick originally had a chick of their
own that was smaller than that of those which did not desert their foster chick. Whether parents deserted their foster chick
was also negatively related to the size of the foster chick. The mass of the foster chick was more important than the size
of the parents' own chick in determining the desertion rate of chicks. The mass gain of the foster chick during the first
5 days after switching was positively related to the body condition of foster parents and also positively related to the mass
of the foster parents' own chick, but negatively related to the size of the foster chick. The results suggest that puffins
adjust their parental effort according to both their own body condition and the size of the chick. The latter may indicate
the chick's prospect of survival and recruitment to the population.
Received: 20 January 1996 / Accepted after revision: 27 October 1996 相似文献
18.
On the evolutionary stability of female infanticide 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
Territoriality among female rodents may have evolved as an adaptation to intraspecific competition for resources or, alternatively,
to defend pups against infanticide. In order to evaluate the latter, we analyse the conditions that allow an infanticidal
strategy to invade a population of non-infanticidal females, and the circumstances under which infanticide may become an evolutionarily
stable strategy (ESS). Our game theoretical analyses indicate that infanticide has to be associated with some direct (cannibalism)
or indirect (reduced competition) resource benefits in order to invade a non-infanticidal population. We also expect that
females will primarily kill litters of nearby neighbors, thereby removing the closest competitors while keeping costs at a
low level. However, once established in a population, infanticide may be an ESS, even if females do not gain any resource
benefits. This is theoretically possible if a female through infanticide can reduce the possibility that other, potentially
infanticidal, females establish and/or stay close to her nest. While behavioral data indicate that these special circumstances
sometimes occur, they may be too specific to apply generally to small rodents. Therefore, we expect that the evolutionary
stability of infanticide often requires resource benefits, and that female infanticide in small rodents may, in fact, be a
consequence rather than a cause of territoriality.
Received: 27 June 1996 / Accepted after revision: 28 December 1996 相似文献
19.
We examined patterns of concurrent multiple mating in a live-bearing poeciliid fish, the sailfin molly (Poecilia latipinna). We tested whether the probability of multiple paternity was related to female body size or fertility and whether the rate
of multiple paternity varied among four populations that differed in their distributions of female body size and fertility.
We analyzed data on mother and offspring genotypes for three polymorphic allozymes by three techniques, including a maximum-likelihood
estimator that accounts for sampling error in both parental and offspring allele frequencies. The estimated rate of multiple
paternity varied between 0.09 and 0.85, and the rate in one population varied seasonally between 0.33 (spring) and 0.85 (autumn).
The variation in these rates was not associated with variation in body-size distributions among populations but was closely
associated with variation in size-specific fertility: populations with greater variation in female fertility had higher multiple-paternity
rates. Within two populations, logistic regression revealed that individual females of larger body size and greater size-specific
fertility were more likely to carry multiply sired broods. This result is consistent with observations made in one of the
populations 5 years earlier. In general, the results strongly suggest that the mating system varies markedly among conspecific
populations of sailfin mollies and that larger, more fertile females are the objects of intermale competition.
Received: 6 May 1996 / Accepted in revised form: 5 December 1996 相似文献
20.
Xavier Lambin 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,40(6):363-372
Numerous accounts of partial or complete breeding dispersal by adult females reported in the literature on microtines have
been interpreted as maternal bequeathal. However, for most species, no causal relationship between a female's movements between
successive litters and the settlement of her offspring in the natal range has been demonstrated. I tested predictions from
the bequeathal hypothesis using data from an intensive study of Townsend's voles where genetical relationship had been determined
by marking most pups in the maternal nest before weaning. After correcting statistically for temporal changes in the distance
moved between successive litters I rejected the following predictions: (1) that females with one or several daughters of reproductive
age should be more likely to disperse or disperse farther between successive births than females without such daughters; (2)
that mothers should be more likely to bequeath their range to their daughters in spring when the latter would gain most from
having ready access to a breeding range; (3) that daughters benefit from their mother abandoning the natal range through an
increase in their probability of recruitment; and (4) that daughters actually use the home range vacated by their mother.
I critically reassess the empirical studies quoted as evidence that breeding dispersal is a form of parental investment in
microtines and other mammals. Most empirical studies cited in support of the bequeathal hypothesis often only contain anecdotal
reports of movements by breeding females or do not mention it as one of its possible adaptive functions. Some studies contain
evidence only consistent with the bequeathal hypothesis whereas others are incompatible with this explanation. Documented
evidence of bequeathal comes exclusively from species which rely on a semi-permanent resource such as a midden, mound or burrow.
I conclude that there is no indication from Townsend's voles or any other microtines that females abandon their breeding range
to their female offspring as a form of parental investment.
Received: 27 February 1996 / Accepted after revision: 1 March 1997 相似文献