共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
The extent of diversity within closely related taxa may be a function of their shared evolutionary history or of selective forces causing adaptive changes. Examining variation among taxa within a single genus may help to identify flexibility in trait variation because recently diverged populations are more likely living in the environment of adaptation. This study examines correlates of diversity in Eulemur, a genus that has a wide distribution in a variety of habitat types throughout Madagascar. Previously published data were gathered from 11 long-term studies of Eulemur populations. Variables were categorized into multiple datasets: (1) environmental characteristics, (2) social organization, and (3) ecology, which included subsets for ranging behavior, diet, and activity budget. Molecular phylogenies from the literature were used to create the fourth and final dataset, a dissimilarity matrix of evolutionary distance among the 12 species and subspecies. Principal components and cluster analyses were implemented to examine the overall ecological similarity among Eulemur populations and to determine which variables contribute most to the variation among taxa. Partial Mantel tests were conducted to test for correlations among the dataset matrices. The results suggest ecological flexibility for the genus, in particular, populations in similar environments displayed similar activity patterns. In contrast, social organization showed no relationship with environment but was correlated with phylogenetic distance among populations. While Eulemur seems to demonstrate some flexibility for ecological adaptations, characteristics related to group size and sex ratio more closely track phylogeny and thus may be less flexible.Electronic supplementary material Supplementary material is available in the online version of this article at and is accessible to authorized users. 相似文献
2.
In monogamous species, females often choose between males according to the quality of the territories they defend, but the
extent to which females themselves contribute to territory defence is frequently underestimated. Here we test for differences
in male and female roles during paired scent-marking bouts, a key component of territorial defence, in a monogamous antelope.
In two populations (Kenya, Zimbabwe) of klipspringer, Oreotragus oreotragus, both males and females usually scent-marked at the same site, but there were significant differences between sexes in terms
of investment within bouts. Females initiated most bouts, thus dictating the marking strategy of the pair. Males initiated
relatively few bouts, but deposited more scent marks per bout than females and were usually the last to scent-mark before
leaving the site; they marked on the same branches as the female and thus overmarked her scent. Both sexes deposited more
marks during paired than solo visits. Immediately preceding and following scent-marking bouts, males approached females and
females left males more often than expected. Female scent-marking rates were higher when they were receptive than at other
times, and this increase was matched by elevated marking rates of males. Females may increase marking rates when they are
receptive in order to test the quality of their mate or to incite male competition. However, these ideas are unlikely to explain
female scent-marking behaviour outside the mating season, which appears to be related primarily to territorial defence. We
suggest that these differences in investment in scent-marking bouts are consistent with predictions that females may be autonomously
territorial and that overmarking of female scent by males is a form of mate-guarding.
Received: 17 November 1999 / Received in revised form: 24 February 2000 / Accepted: 13 March 2000 相似文献
3.
We report on the genetic evaluation and behavioral study of social organization in the Asian elephant (Elephas maximus). Although Asian elephants and African elephants (Loxodonta africana) were previously thought to have similar social organizations, our results demonstrate a substantial difference in the complexity
and structure of Asian elephant social groupings from that described for African savanna elephants. Photographic cataloging
of individuals, radio telemetry, and behavioral observations in Ruhuna National Park, Sri Lanka, enabled us to assign associated
females and young to four groups with overlapping ranges. Genetic sampling of individuals from the four groups in Ruhuna National
Park and three other groups in surrounding areas, conducted through PCR amplification and sequencing of mitochondrial DNA
from dung, supported the matriarchal nature of female groups and the lack of inter-group transfer of females. Behaviorally
and genetically, the identified social groups were best described as ”family groups”. We did not find any evidence for the
existence of social groups of higher complexity than family groups.
Received: 25 March 2000 / Received in revised form: 28 March 2000 / Accepted: 1 April 2000 相似文献
4.
Variation of the amplification effect of burrows of the leptodactylid frog Eupsophus emiliopugini on conspecific calls generated externally was investigated. Advertisement calls broadcast through a loudspeaker placed in the vicinity of a burrow were monitored with small microphones positioned inside and outside the cavity. For 150 presentations of calls of 15 individuals in 12 burrows, 134 were amplified and 16 were attenuated (range –6–13 dB). The fundamental resonant frequency of burrows, measured with broadcast noise and pure tones, averaged 814 Hz (range 302–1361 Hz) and covaried with burrow length. The dominant frequency of the calls of burrow occupants (average 1062 Hz, range 636–1459 Hz) was not correlated with the fundamental resonant frequency of these cavities. In burrows with low resonant frequencies, externally broadcast calls with high dominant frequencies were attenuated, or amplified to a lower extent than calls with lower dominant frequencies. The dominant frequencies of the calls experienced shifts towards the burrows’ fundamental resonant frequencies. The amplification of calls inside burrows of E. emiliopugini exhibits manifest variability, with considerable potential for facilitating acoustic interactions in this species. Received: 18 July 1999 / Received in revised form: 19 July 1999 / Accepted: 25 July 1999 相似文献
5.
Testosterone and the allocation of reproductive effort in male house finches (Carpodacus mexicanus) 总被引:8,自引:1,他引:8
Testosterone has been proposed to serve as the mediator that controls the relative effort that an individual male bird will
devote to mating effort versus parental effort. Here, we demonstrate a testosterone-influenced trade-off between parental
and mating efforts in male house finches. Male house finches with experimentally elevated testosterone fed nestlings at a
significantly lower rate, but sang at a higher rate than males without manipulated testosterone levels. Females mated to testosterone-implanted
males fed nestlings at a significantly higher rate than females mated to males without testosterone implants, resulting in
similar feeding rates for both treated and untreated pairs. The effects of testosterone on male house finches, however, were
not as dramatic as the effects of testosterone observed in some other socially monogamous species of birds. Because extra-pair
copulations are uncommon in house finches and males provide substantial amounts of parental care, these more modest effects
may be due to differences in how the allocation of reproductive effort affects the costs and benefits of different reproductive
behaviors.
Received: 6 June 2000 / Accepted: 17 July 2000 相似文献
6.
T. Giray Z.-Y. Huang Ernesto Guzmán-Novoa G. E. Robinsons 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1999,47(1-2):17-28
Two factors that influence age at onset of foraging in honeybees are juvenile hormone (JH) and colony age demography (older bees inhibit behavioral development of younger bees). We tested the hypothesis that genetic variation among bees for these factors influences genetic variation in behavioral development. Pairs of colonies showing genetic differences in rates of behavioral development were identified in a screening experiment and bees from these colonies were used for physiological and behavioral assays. Six pairs were assayed, three with European bees only and three with both European and Africanized bees. There was genetic variation for the following four components: (1) production of JH in four pairs (experiment 1); (2) sensitivity to JH in three pairs (experiment 2); (3) sensitivity to social inhibition in three pairs (experiment 3), and (4) potency of social inhibition in four pairs (experiment 4). Cross-fostering assays (experiment 5), which allowed all four components to be evaluated simultaneously, revealed genetic variation for production of JH, sensitivity to JH, or sensitivity to social inhibition in five of six pairs, and potency of social inhibition in five of six pairs. There was often evidence for genotypic differences in more than one component, and no consistent pattern of association among any of the components. Africanized bees had faster rates of behavioral development than European bees, but there were no racial differences in patterns of variation among the four components. These results indicate that there are at least several, apparently distinct, physiological processes associated with JH and colony age demography upon which natural selection can act to alter the rate of behavioral development in honeybees. Received: 8 December 1998 / Received in revised form: 29 July 1999 / Accepted: 8 August 1999 相似文献
7.
The effects of testosterone on antibody production and plumage coloration in male house sparrows (Passer domesticus) 总被引:6,自引:3,他引:6
Many bird species have patches of colour in their plumage, contrasting with their basic coloration, which are used to display
and signal status to conspecifics. These are called ’badges of status’, because they are believed to be low-cost signals of
social status. For a signalling system to be evolutionarily stable, cheating must be controlled. The conventional view is
that there is frequent testing, which uncovers cheats. Recently, the immunocompetence handicap hypothesis (ICHH) suggested
that signals may be dependent on testosterone for their development, with a cost being imposed through immune suppression.
We report experiments on house sparrows (Passer domesticus) which show that testosterone significantly influences the size of the bib (a ’badge of status’). The ultimate effect of
the testosterone manipulation was to impair antibody production, as predicted by the ICHH. However, testosterone manipulations
also changed the levels of the ’stress hormone’ corticosterone. The level of corticosterone was also related to the degree
of immunosuppression. After controlling for the effect of corticosterone, testosterone enhanced the birds’ ability to produce
antibodies, counter to the ICHH. The hypothesis therefore must be modified. We suggest that testosterone has a dual effect:
it leads to immunosuppression through a mechanism involving corticosterone but, conversely, leads to increased immunocompetence
probably via dominance influencing access to resources.
Received: 5 March 1999 / Received in revised form: 1 October 1999 / Accepted: 16 October 1999 相似文献
8.
Allison J. Abell 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,41(4):217-226
The association between spatial proximity and paternity was studied in a population of the striped plateau lizard, Sceloporus virgatus. The relationship between estimated mating success and male phenotypic traits was examined for a sample of 55 males. DNA
samples were obtained from 13 female-offspring families. The males with the closest spatial proximity to each female were
tested as possible sires within each family. Fingerprinting with two multilocus hypervariable minisatellite probes revealed
a strong correspondence between male-female spatial proximity and actual paternity. Paternity could be assigned for 72 of
the 100 hatchlings. Most hatchlings with identifiable sires were attributed to a male with the highest category of spatial
proximity to the mother. However, there was a low to moderate level of multiple paternity within clutches, and for some clutches
probable sires could not be identified even though the most likely behavioural candidates were tested. Thus, nonterritorial
males or other males lacking strong social and spatial relationships with females may achieve some degree of reproductive
success. Analysis of mating success revealed that male success increased with body size, up to a point beyond which larger
size conferred no advantage.
Received: 7 January 1997 / Accepted after revision: 16 June 1997 相似文献
9.
How resources and encounters affect the distribution of foraging activity in a seed-harvesting ant 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
We examined how the foraging ecology of the seed-harvesting ant Messor andrei depends upon the distribution of resources and the presence of conspecifics. Bait experiments showed that colonies can recruit
to high-density patches of seeds. However, at the seasonal scale, natural resource distribution did not affect the distribution
of foraging activity. We conducted the study in years of high rainfall and thus seed availability may not have been a limiting
factor. Colonies always preferred to forage in areas closer to their nest, which may reduce travel time between the nest and
foraging sites. On a day-to-day scale, encounters between neighboring colonies at a site increased the probability that colonies
would return to forage at that site; this was true both for natural and experimental encounters. In the summer, this resulted
in colonies foraging at the sites of intraspecific encounters on more days than in areas where no encounter had occurred.
Encounters between colonies included fighting, and there was little overlap between the foraging areas of neighboring colonies:
both results suggest that one function of encounters is to defend foraging space. The high probability of return to the site
of an encounter between colonies suggests that encounters may have a second function: to indicate the presence of resources.
Received: 28 June 1999 / Received in revised form: 12 October 1999 / Accepted: 16 October 1999 相似文献
10.
C. Cordero 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2000,48(6):458-462
The differential costs of mating paid by males and females influence the nature and strength of sexual selection. In butterflies,
males invest a relatively large amount of time and resources in each mating, but male survival costs of mating have not been
demonstrated. I present the results of experiments designed to measure the effect of different aspects of mating on male longevity
in the polygynous butterfly Callophrys xami. In experiment 1, I compared the longevity of pairs of males that produced similar amounts of spermatophore, but that mated
at different rates, a different numbers of times, and that produced spermatophores at different rates, and found that the
longevity of ”low-mating-rate” males was not different from that of ”high-mating-rate” males. In experiment 2, the longevity
of virgin males was not significantly different from that of multiply mated males. In experiment 3, I used resource-limited
males resulting from experimental food limitation of last-instar larvae; resource-limited virgin males lived significantly
more days than resource-limited multiply mated males. Since ecological costs of mating (e.g., disease transmission, predation
risk) were excluded in the experiment, diminished male longevity was a product of physiological costs of sexual interactions.
These results suggest that the cost of ejaculate production is an important cause of longevity reduction when there are resource
limitations; however, the role of other possible physiological costs of mating in longevity reduction is still unknown.
Received: 21 March 2000 / Accepted: 26 August 2000 相似文献
11.
Recent models of choosiness in mate choice have identified two particularly important factors: the potential reproductive
rate (PRR) of the choosing sex relative to that of the chosen sex, and the variation in quality of potential mates. This experimental
study tested how these factors affected choosiness in male and female sand gobies, Pomatoschistus minutus. We manipulated relative PRR by means of water temperature, and mate quality by means of body length. The choosing male or
female was offered a choice between two mates with either a small or a large difference in body length representing a small
or a large variation in mate quality. Choosiness was measured as (1) preference for the larger mate, and (2) as whether or
not spawning occurred with the smaller mate, while the larger mate was visible but screened off. We found that females preferred
large males, and that their level of choosiness was affected by variation in male quality, but not by their own relative PRR.
Males, on the other hand, seemed unselective in all treatments and were in general more likely than females to spawn with
their provided partner. This suggests that in the sand goby, variation in male mate quality has a greater influence than relative
PRR on facultative changes in female choosiness. However, a general difference in PRR between males and females may be one
important factor explaining the observed sex difference in choosiness.
Received: 17 April 2000 / Revised: 24 June 2000 / Accepted: 17 July 2000 相似文献
12.
J.C. Mitani W.J. Sanders J.S. Lwanga T.L. Windfelder 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2001,49(2-3):187-195
Evaluating the nature and significance of predation on populations of wild primates has been difficult given a paucity of
data regarding the phenomenon. Here we addressed this problem in a 37-month study of the predatory behavior of crowned hawk-eagles
living at the Ngogo study site in Kibale National Park, Uganda. We collected prey remains underneath the nests of two pairs
of eagles and census data on potential prey species to investigate prey selection and the ecological impact of predation on
the Ngogo primate population. Results indicate that primates form the vast majority of all prey items. Eagles prey selectively
on monkeys according to sex and species. Male primates were taken more often than females, while two species, redtail monkeys
and mangabeys, were captured significantly more and less, respectively, than chance expectation. In addition, there was no
bias in the age of prey: adult and non-adults were killed in numbers roughly equal to their proportional representations in
the forest. Further analyses indicate that a non-trivial fraction of the entire primate population at Ngogo succumbs to crowned
hawk-eagle predation each year. These results reveal both parallels and contrasts with those reported previously. Some of
the parallels are due to similarities in prey availability, while contrasts are likely related to methodological differences
between studies, inter- individual variations in predator hunting styles, and differences in prey abundance, demography, and
behavior.
Received: 29 March 2000 / Revised: 6 June 2000 / Accepted: 15 October 2000 相似文献
13.
Sperm characteristics associated with different male reproductive tactics in bluegills (Lepomis macrochirus) 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
We examined the availability and motility of sperm from parental and sneaker male bluegills (Lepomis macrochirus), a colonially nesting sunfish (Family Centrarchidae) with male parental care and a high incidence of cuckoldry by both sneaker
and satellite males. We found no differences between sneakers and parentals in length and swimming speed of sperm, or percent
and duration of sperm activity. In sneaker milt, however, sperm was almost 50% more concentrated than in parental milt (16.5×106 vs 11.5×106 sperm/μl of milt, respectively). Despite this difference in sperm concentration, stripped ejaculates from sneakers contained
almost 400 million fewer sperm (only 32% as many sperm) than those from parentals due to their much smaller stripped ejaculate
volumes (only about 19% that of parentals). Thus unless sneakers can compensate by releasing more sperm or gaining closer
proximity to eggs at the time of spawning, they may be at a disadvantage with respect to sperm competition. We discuss these
results in relation to models for the evolution of alternative reproductive behaviours in this species and suggest that the
cuckolders may be making the best of a bad situation.
Received: 18 February 2000 / Revised: 23 March 2000 / Accepted: 14 September 2000 相似文献
14.
A. Peters L. B. Astheimer C. R. J. Boland A. Cockburn 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2000,47(6):438-445
Testosterone has been proposed as a physiological link between the level of sexual signalling and male condition. Bright plumage
is one of the most noticeable sexual signals and is often used by females as a basis for mate choice. Yet bright male plumage
is not necessarily testosterone dependent. We investigated the role of testosterone in the moult into seasonal nuptial plumage
in male superb fairy-wrens. Early pre-nuptial moult is under intense intersexual selection and males can acquire the bright
plumage any time between autumn and the next spring. Testosterone was always undetectable or very low in males in dull eclipse
plumage. During the pre-nuptial moult, both the number of males with detectable testosterone and average testosterone levels
increased sharply. High testosterone was more correlated with nuptial plumage than with presence of the cloacal protuberance
(indicative of sperm storage). Subcutaneous testosterone implants always induced the pre-nuptial moult within 2–3 weeks after
implantation, even well outside the natural time range of moulting. Moreover, removal of the implants before the nuptial plumage
was completed, arrested the moult process. The evidence suggests that development of the nuptial plumage is testosterone dependent,
although we cannot exclude that testosterone exerts its action after conversion to a metabolite such as oestrogen. Once the
nuptial plumage was completed, all males maintained substantially elevated testosterone, sometimes months before the onset
of breeding. These high levels could be necessary to maintain the plumage, and/or are involved in courtship displays. The
results are discussed with respect to potential costs involved in acquiring and maintaining the nuptial plumage.
Received: 17 January 2000 / Received in revised form: 24 February 2000 / Accepted: 25 February 2000 相似文献
15.
We conducted playback experiments to examine how parent tree swallows (Tachycineta bicolor) use nestling begging calls to distribute feedings to individuals within broods. In a first study, we used a paired-choice
test to determine if parents discriminated between the taped begging calls of nestlings deprived of food and those of nestlings
that had been recently fed. Our results showed that parents directed their first feeding attempt towards model nestlings near
speakers playing deprived calls significantly more often than to models near speakers playing fed calls. They also made more
feeding attempts overall to models with deprived calls. In the second study, we varied call rate and amplitude to examine
which call features parents might use to discriminate begging calls. Parents directed significantly more first feeding attempts
and more feeding attempts overall towards non-begging nestlings near speakers playing high call rates than to nestlings near
speakers playing low call rates. They did not, however, discriminate between calls differing in amplitude. Previous studies
have shown that parent birds use begging calls to regulate overall feeding rates to the brood. Our results suggest that parent
tree swallows also use begging calls when feeding individual nestlings and, in particular, prefer calls associated with increased
levels of nestling hunger.
Received: 14 February 2000 / Revised: 6 October 2000 / Accepted: 16 October 2000 相似文献
16.
A recently favored hypothesis is that duetting in birds has a mate-guarding function: a male responds vocally to his partner’s
song, thereby forming a duet that repels males who are attracted to her song. Previous studies have not provided unambiguous
tests of the mate-guarding hypothesis because: (1) the probability of a male answering his partner’s song has not been shown
to increase specifically when the female is fertile, and (2) the probability of a male answering his partner’s song has not
been assessed separately from simply a higher song initiation rate. We investigated extra-pair paternity, mate-guarding, and
duetting in the socially monogamous Australian magpie-lark (Grallina cyanoleuca). DNA fingerprinting revealed that 3% of young were the result of extra-pair paternity, and we found that males guarded fertile
females by staying close to them. However, males did not initiate songs at a higher rate when females were fertile and actually
reduced their probability of replying to female song during this period. We conclude that although male magpie-larks did guard
fertile females in an attempt to prevent extra-pair copulations, they did not use duetting for this purpose.
Received: 10 May 1999 / Received in revised form: 27 September 1999 / Accepted: 2 October 1999 相似文献
17.
Multiple-queen (polygyne) colonies of the introduced fire ant Solenopsis invicta present a paradox for kin selection theory. Egg-laying queens within these societies are, on average, unrelated to one another,
and the numbers of queens per colony are high, so that workers appear to raise new sexuals that are no more closely related
to them than are random individuals in the population. This paradox could be resolved if workers discriminate between related
and unrelated nestmate sexuals in important fitness-related contexts. This study examines the possibility of such nepotism
using methods that combine the following features: (1) multiple relevant behavioral assays, (2) colonies with an unmanipulated
family structure, (3) multiple genetic markers with no known phenotypic effects, and (4) a statistical technique for distinguishing
between nepotism and potentially confounding phenomena. We estimated relatedness between interactants in polygyne S. invicta colonies in two situations, workers tending egg-laying queens and workers feeding maturing winged queens. In neither case
did we detect a significant positive value of relatedness that would implicate nepotism. We argue that the non-nepotistic
strategies displayed by these ants reflect historical selection pressures experienced by native populations, in which nestmate
queens are highly related to one another. The markedly different genetic structure in native populations may favor the operation
of stronger higher-level selection that effectively opposes weaker individual-level selection for nepotistic interactions
within nests.
Received: 28 June 1996 / Accepted after revision: 6 October 1996 相似文献
18.
Neil J. Buckley 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,41(4):267-279
In field tests of the information-center hypothesis (ICH) in south Texas with black vultures (Coragyps atratus) and turkey vultures (Cathartes aura), large carcasses were provided and kept under continuous observation. The use vultures made of these bait sites and their
patterns of arrival were recorded to evaluate predictions derived from the ICH. Turkey vultures discovered most bait sites
(30 of 31) first, but frequently were displaced from the food by later-arriving black vultures. This competitive exclusion
by black vultures limited subsequent feeding opportunities for turkey vultures sufficiently that few (27%) returned on subsequent
days to bait sites they had previously visited. I found no evidence that those turkey vultures that did return to bait sites
acted as leaders for groups of naive birds and led them to bait sites – knowledgeable and naive turkey vultures did not arrive
at bait sites together, and groups arriving at bait sites were not larger on subsequent days than on the first days carcasses
were available. In contrast, a significantly larger percentage (47%) of knowledgeable black vultures returned to bait sites
they had visited on previous days, and the first groups of black vultures arriving at bait sites on subsequent days were significantly
larger than the equivalent groups on first days. Nine flocks of black vultures that arrived on subsequent days at bait sites
before sunrise (which suggests the birds had commuted directly from a roost) contained knowledgeable birds, and two of these
flocks contained both knowledgeable and naive individuals. Overall, 10 of 54 naive tagged black vultures (18.5%) arrived at
bait sites under circumstances that suggested they had followed conspecifics to the food from a roost. However, most black
vultures apparently found carcasses through independent search or by using local enhancement. Therefore, I conclude that while
following from roosts to food sites is a strategy used by black vultures, at this study site it is one they use relatively
infrequently.
Received: 20 February 1997 / Accepted after revision: 28 June 1997 相似文献
19.
M. J. O’Riain N. C. Bennett P. N. M. Brotherton G. McIlrath T. H. Clutton-Brock 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2000,48(6):471-477
Meerkats live in co-operatively breeding familial groups in which reproduction is monopolised by a dominant pair of breeders.
Offspring of the breeders are behaviourally subordinate, and typically remain in their natal group as sexually mature, non-breeding
helpers. In this study, we investigated the proximate factors limiting subordinate reproduction. Evidence for reproductive
suppression by dominants was investigated by comparing life history, behaviour and hormonal profiles of dominants and subordinates.
Baseline levels of plasma luteinising hormone (LH) were significantly higher in dominant than in subordinate females. However,
following an exogenous injection of gonadotrophin-releasing hormone (GnRH), both categories had comparable concentrations
of circulating LH. There were no significant differences in pre- or post-GnRH challenge LH levels in dominant or subordinate
males. Reproduction in both dominant and subordinate females rarely occurred in the absence of unrelated males. Given that
groups typically comprise parents and offspring, lack of suitable mates emerged as the primary constraint on subordinate reproduction.
When this constraint was removed, subordinates typically bred but at a lower rate than dominants. This difference in reproduction
may be attributed to intrasexual competition manifested through direct interference by dominant females through subordinate
evictions, infanticide and the abandoning of subordinate litters. We argue that differences in reproductive regulation within
mammalian co-operative breeding systems may be explained by differences in the mating strategy (inbreeding versus outbreeding)
and the probability that subordinates in obligate outbreeding species will encounter unrelated opposite-sex partners.
Received: 19 April 2000 / Accepted: 17 July 2000 相似文献
20.
T. R. Pope 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2000,48(4):253-267
Evaluation of evolutionary mechanisms proposed to promote cooperative behavior depends on the relative influence of the behavior
on the reproductive success of individuals, the reproductive success of the group in which they interact behaviorally, and
the degree of gene correlation among cooperators. The genetic relationship within cooperative coalitions of female red howler
monkeys was examined for three populations with different densities and growth rates. Patterns of gene correlation change
within coalitions is documented using data from the mitochondrial and nuclear genomes, and long-term census monitoring. Differences
in fecundity and infant survivorship within and between groups of unrelated (rˉ=0) and related (rˉ≥ 0.25) females are compared. Females that emigrate from their natal groups form coalitions with other migrant females. These
coalitions attempt to establish a territory and, once successful in producing offspring, exclude other females from feeding
resources. Females in these coalitions had different mtDNA haplotypes and a genetically estimated mean r of 0, supporting demographic data on emigration patterns indicating that these females rarely have the opportunity to form
coalitions with kin. Patterns of recruitment and rate of matriline development within social groups supported behavioral data
indicating that females actively attempt to promote their own matriline as breeders over that of other females, and that some
matrilines are more successful at this than others. Mean r among females was significantly higher in coalitions established as social groups for several generations (rˉ=0.44). In these groups, females all shared the same mtDNA haplotype, and mtDNA haplotype divergence was significantly higher
between than within groups. Females in coalitions with kin had significantly higher reproductive success than females in unrelated
coalitions in all populations. This difference was not a function of coalition size, number of males, socionomic sex ratio,
or primiparity, although anecdotal evidence suggests that allomothering may compensate for inept new mothers in related coalitions
more often than in unrelated ones. Differences in territory quality could not be ruled out as a potential causal factor in
the saturated populations, but were unlikely in the low-density, growing population. There were substantial differences among
long-established coalitions in overall reproductive output in all three populations, and this was significantly correlated
with the number of breeding females. Increase in coalition size was a function of both group age and the behavioral tolerance
among females. Regardless of the underlying reasons for the patterns observed, reproductive success clearly increases with
degree of gene correlation among females within cooperative coalitions, and coalitions that recruit more daughters produce
more offspring. The nature of the cooperative relationship among group females directly influences both of these outcomes.
This is associated with substantial genetic differentiation among social groups within populations, creating conditions in
which genetic tendencies towards cooperative behavior can become tightly associated with group reproductive success.
Received: 15 September 1999 / Revised: 27 April 2000 / Accepted: 27 May 2000 相似文献