共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
Elizabeth A. Langridge Ana B. Sendova-Franks Nigel R. Franks 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2008,62(3):447-456
Certain groups of organisms are capable of improving their collective performance with experience. In a recent study, we demonstrated
that, over successive emigrations, colonies of the ant Temnothorax albipennis are able to improve their collective performance by reducing the time taken to complete an emigration (Langridge et al.,
Behav Ecol Sociobiol 56:523–529, 2004). In this paper, by recording the performance of individually marked workers during
repeated emigrations, we were able to analyse some of the ways in which time gains are achieved. We found that: (1) those
transporters that also transported in the preceding emigration began to transport earlier in the current emigration and, in
the majority of emigrations, transported more items than those transporters that had not transported in the preceding emigration;
(2) the time that elapsed before the first item was transported into the new nest reduced over successive emigrations, and
this first item was, in the majority of emigrations, carried by a transporter that had also transported in the preceding emigration;
(3) the number of adults that were transported reduced over successive emigrations. Our results strongly suggest that the
behaviour of transporters that also transported in a preceding emigration may be modified as a result of their experience
and that, consequently, their efforts in the next emigration make a major contribution to the improved performance of the
colony as a whole. 相似文献
2.
Elizabeth A. Langridge Ana B. Sendova-Franks Nigel R. Franks 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2008,62(12):1851-1861
Colonies of the ant Temnothorax albipennis improve their collective performance over successive emigrations (Langridge et al. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 56:523–529, 2004,
Behav Ecol Sociobiol 62:447–456, 2008). Here, by analysing the performance of individual transporters (workers that carry
the brood, queen and a proportion of adults), we investigate whether they spend less time at the old and new nests during
repeated emigrations. Transporters expedited choosing and picking up brood items at the old nest and depositing them in the
new nest. Such improvements were not associated with adult transport. Generally, when carrying brood items, but not when carrying
adults, transporters visited several locations in the new nest before depositing them. Transporters did not interact with
other adults when depositing brood items. Consequently, reductions in depositing times are the sum of time savings made by
individual transporters. By contrast, transporters spent most time interacting with other adults before picking up brood items
at the old nest. As the frequency of these interactions did not decline, we suggest the behaviours of interacting adults were
modified in a way that hastened their completion. Thus, reductions in picking-up times probably occur because of time saved
during interactions. 相似文献
3.
Audrey Dussutour Stamatios C. Nicolis Jean-Louis Deneubourg Vincent Fourcassié 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2006,61(1):17-30
In this paper we examine the effect of crowding on the selection of a path in the mass-recruiting ant Lasius niger. In our experiment, ants had to go from their nest to a food source by crossing a diamond-shaped bridge, giving the choice between two paths. Two types of bridges were used: the first had two branches of equal length but different width while the second had two branches of different length and width. Experiments at high traffic volume always ended up with the selection of the wider branch, even if it was longer. This result shows that overcrowding on the narrow branch plays an essential role in the mechanism underlying the choice of route in ants. A mathematical model was developed to evaluate the importance of two mechanisms that could account for this result. The first is based on the difference in travel duration between the two paths. The second is based on the repulsive interactions between workers making head-on encounters. The model shows that travel duration per se is not sufficient to explain path choice. Rather, it is the interplay between trail following behaviour and repulsive interactions that allows ants to choose the path that minimizes their travel time. When choosing a path ants thus prefer to trade time against energy. Our results demonstrate that any environmental constraint that alters the dynamics of trail recruitment can lead to the emergence of adaptive foraging decisions without any explicit coding of information by the foragers at the individual level. 相似文献
4.
Summary. Because generalist ants are aggressive towards foreign insects, the recognition of homopterans by tending ants is critical
in ant/homopteran trophobiosis. Herein we report experimental evidence indicating that Argentine ant, Linepithema humile (Mayr) (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) learn to associate the production of honeydew with the chemical characteristics of homopteran
cuticle, suppressing ant aggression and allowing the ants to tend homopterans. Although chemically-mediated associative learning
is well understood in honeybee foraging, to our knowledge, it has not been reported before in ant/homopteran trophobiosis. 相似文献
5.
Harald Wolf 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2008,62(3):415-425
In the habitat of desert ants, Cataglyphis fortis, a constant wind is usually blowing during the daytime. When visiting a familiar food source, the ants steer some distance
downwind of the feeder, rather than attempting a direct approach that might miss small food sources, in particular. In the
downwind area, the ants pick up the odor plume emanating from the food and follow it upwind to the prey. This strategy saves
considerable walking distance and time. The additional path necessitated by the downwind strategy is only about 0.75 to 2 m,
depending on nest–feeder distance, while missing the food on the upwind side results in much longer search trajectories. During
the initial three to five visits to a feeding site, downwind distance and length of the approach path are shortened notably,
and the approach trajectory is straightened. Desert ants further exhibit considerable short-term flexibility in their approach.
Experienced individuals are evidently able to decide upon leaving the nest which direction to choose toward the feeder, depending
on current wind direction (that fluctuates slightly during the day). Notable changes in wind direction occur primarily overnight.
For larger nest–feeder distances, the animals adjust their approach en route to the altered wind direction during their first
foraging trip in the morning. 相似文献
6.
Samuel R. Scholes Ana B. Sendova-Franks S. Tim Swift Chris Melhuish 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2006,59(4):531-540
A fundamental issue of collective intelligence is whether the collective pattern or process is based on environmental information
that explicitly codes for it or arises through self-organization of the individuals. Sometimes, these alternatives occur together.
Adaptive systems may also be capable of utilizing different types of mechanism under different conditions. Sendova-Franks
et al. (Anim Behav 68:1095–1106, 2004) demonstrated evidence for a self-organization mechanism of brood sorting in the ant
Temnothorax albipennis, where the brood are sorted in a series of bands or concentric annuli that increase in size with distance from the colony
centre. The work by Cox and Blanchard (J Theor Biol 204:223-238, 2000) suggests an alternative or complementary mechanism
whereby the brood pattern is specified by the template of a CO2 gradient. Here, we test for a gaseous template as a necessary condition for brood sorting. Under the experimental condition,
we pumped the air out of the nest continuously to prevent the accumulation of any gaseous substances. We compared the brood
pattern between the experimental and control conditions according to four characteristics: mean distance from centre, mean
nearest-neighbour distance, shape and area. Under the experimental condition, the order of brood types according to the first
two characteristics was the same as in the control. The area of the brood pattern was smaller, and its shape elongated under
the experimental condition. As expected on the basis of these differences, mean distance from centre was greater and mean
nearest-neighbour distance was smaller under the experimental condition (although not statistically significantly) and by
the expected amount. We found evidence that ants avoid placing brood in the strongest airflow stream. This could explain the
reduced area and elongated shape of the brood pattern under the experimental condition. We conclude that a gaseous template
is not a necessary condition for brood sorting.
Electronic supplementary material Supplementary material is available in the online version of this article at and is accessible for authorized users. 相似文献
7.
Elva J. H. Robinson Thomas O. Richardson Ana B. Sendova-Franks Ofer Feinerman Nigel R. Franks 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2009,63(5):779-636
Ant colonies are factories within fortresses (Oster and Wilson 1978). They run on resources foraged from an outside world fraught with danger. On what basis do individual ants decide to leave the safety of the nest? We investigated the relative roles of social information (returning nestmates), individual experience and physiology (lipid stores/corpulence) in predicting which ants leave the nest and when. We monitored Temnothorax albipennis workers individually using passive radio-frequency identification technology, a novel procedure as applied to ants. This method allowed the matching of individual corpulence measurements to activity patterns of large numbers of individuals over several days. Social information and physiology are both good predictors of when an ant leaves the nest. Positive feedback from social information causes bouts of activity at the colony level. When certain social information is removed from the system by preventing ants returning, physiology best predicts which ants leave the nest and when. Individual experience is strongly related to physiology. A small number of lean individuals are responsible for most external trips. An individual’s nutrient status could be a useful cue in division of labour, especially when public information from other ants is unavailable. 相似文献
8.
Mutualistic relationships between ants and aphids are well studied but it is unknown if aphid-attending ants place a greater relative importance on defending aphids from aphid-predators or from competing ant colonies. We tested the hypothesis that aphid-attending ants defend their aphids against aphid-predators more aggressively than against ants from neighboring colonies. We conducted introduction trials by placing an individual non-predatory insect, an aphid-predator, or a foreign conspecific ant on the leaf of a resident ant. We found that ants did not attack non-predatory insects, but did attack competing ants and aphid-predators. When we presented resident ants with both the threats (i.e., predator and competitor) at the same time, residents always attacked potential competitors as opposed to aphid-predators. We suggest this behavior may reduce the likelihood of raids by neighboring colonies. Ants appear to balance both the energetic costs of making an attack and the costs associated with losing aphids to a predator, against the benefits of signaling their defensive ability to rivals and/or preventing rivals from gaining knowledge of a potential food resource. 相似文献
9.
Desert ants, Cataglyphis fortis, return to their nest by means of path integration vectors. By using the reversal of these vectors, they approach previously
visited feeding sites again. They adjust these vectors whenever outbound and inbound vector are set into conflict or when
they make use of external cues. Here, we examine the influence of repeated training on the accuracy, precision, and straightness
of outbound and inbound vectors. We trained desert ants to forage to and fro between their nest and a feeder and made sure
that they relied exclusively on their path integrator. Neither the ants’ outbound nor their inbound runs, which, in general,
are straighter than the outbound runs, become more accurate, precise, or straighter during repeated training. Hence, repeated
training does not improve the path integrator in desert ants. 相似文献
10.
Joo Rodrigues 《Ecological modelling》2007,200(3-4):384-392
In this paper we present a model that determines the number of potential recruits of Lasius niger, when feeding on a liquid sugar source. The core of the model are two rules: (i) there is a number of workers (facultative foragers) that become potential recruits if starved and (ii) facultative foragers are more likely to become starved than nonforagers because they are more likely to donate food in a trophallaxis (sugar-exchange) event. We develop and explore an analytical model based on these rules, deriving the number of potential recruits after an arbitrary period of starvation. We develop a simplified recruitment model and observe that the predictions of the model are in rough agreement with the empirical data. 相似文献
11.
Abstract In a foraging column of the leaf-cutting ant Atta cephalotes, minim workers (the smallest worker subcaste) hitchhike on leaf fragments carried by larger workers. It has been demonstrated that they defend leaf carriers against parasitic phorid flies. The present study examines the cues used by the potential hitchhikers to locate leaf carriers. As recently reported, foraging workers stridulate while cutting a leaf fragment, and the stridulatory vibrations serve as closerange recruitment signals. We tested the hypothesis that these plant-borne stridulatory vibrations are used by the potential hitchhikers to locate workers engaged in cutting. Three different lines of evidence support this view. Firstly, the repetition rate of the stridulations produced by foraging workers increases significantly as foragers maneuver the leaf fragment into the carrying position and walk loaded to the nest. This is the moment when hitchhikers usually climb on the leaf. Although the leaf-borne stridulatory vibrations are considerably attenuated when transmitted through the workers' legs, they can nevertheless be detected at short distances by minims. This subcaste is several times more sensitive to substrate-borne vibrations than larger workers. Secondly, when a stridulating and a silent leaf were simultaneously presented at the foraging site, minim workers spent significantly more time on the stridulating than on the silent leaf. Thirdly, hitchhiking was more frequent in leaf carriers which cut fragments out of the stridulating leaf than in those cutting the silent leaf.Abstract In a foraging column of the leaf-cutting ant Atta cephalotes, minim workers (the smallest worker subcaste) hitchhike on leaf fragments carried by larger workers. It has been demonstrated that they defend leaf carriers against parasitic phorid flies. The present study examines the cues used by the potential hitchhikers to locate leaf carriers. As recently reported, foraging workers stridulate while cutting a leaf fragment, and the stridulatory vibrations serve as closerange recruitment signals. We tested the hypothesis that these plant-borne stridulatory vibrations are used by the potential hitchhikers to locate workers engaged in cutting. Three different lines of evidence support this view. Firstly, the repetition rate of the stridulations produced by foraging workers increases significantly as foragers maneuver the leaf fragment into the carrying position and walk loaded to the nest. This is the moment when hitchhikers usually climb on the leaf. Although the leaf-borne stridulatory vibrations are considerably attenuated when transmitted through the workers' legs, they can nevertheless be detected at short distances by minims. This subcaste is several times more sensitive to substrate-borne vibrations than larger workers. Secondly, when a stridulating and a silent leaf were simultaneously presented at the foraging site, minim workers spent significantly more time on the stridulating than on the silent leaf. Thirdly, hitchhiking was more frequent in leaf carriers which cut fragments out of the stridulating leaf than in those cutting the silent leaf.Communicated by P. Pamilo 相似文献
12.
Bioassays were performed to investigate and compare the toxic properties of the contact venoms of three Neotropical Crematogaster species (C. sp. prox. abstinens, C. distans and C. brevispinosa rochai), whose main venom compounds are chemically different (long chain derivatives linked to an aldehyde or a primary acetate,
and furanocembrenoid diterpenes, respectively). Different quantities of venom were topically applied on the bodies of three
target insect species (workers of the termite Nasutitermes ephratae, workers of the ant Solenopsis sp., “media” and “major” workers of the ant Cephalotes pusillus). The toxicity of the venom greatly varied according to the Crematogaster species, the venom of C. b. rochai showing the highest toxic activity towards all target species. The sensitivity of the target species also greatly varied,
the workers of N. ephratae showing a great sensitivity to the venoms, while the heavily armoured workers of the ant C. pusillus, particularly “major” ones, were resistant. Cuticle thickness was found to be a key factor in the resistance to the toxic
effects of Crematogaster venom. Sensitivity of the target workers was also directly related to the amount of venom applied to their cuticle. Workers
of C. distans were resistant to topical application of their own venom, while C. sp. prox. abstinens and C. b. rochai were not. In all cases, the venoms were slow acting, as several hours to one or even several days were needed to obtain a
significant mortality. The main biological activities of the Crematogaster venom could be related to more immediate repellent properties, as recently shown with the European species C. scutellaris. 相似文献
13.
The allocation to growth, defense and reproduction varies in social insects within a species' life cycle and between species. A life cycle model (Oster and Wilson 1978) generally failed to predict caste allocation in small litter-nesting colonies of Neotropical Pheidole. Two of its assumptions were often invalid: food was unlikely to be limiting in four of five populations, and sexual biomass production accelerated, not decelerated, with colony size in three of five populations. One of five Pheidole populations studied had higher caste ratios (soldiers /workers) in reproductive colonies as predicted, and in no species did caste functions conform to predictions. We also adapted three models from plant defense theory to study between-species patterns of caste allocation. Among 12 litter Pheidole the amount of sterile biomass devoted to soldiers varied from 18 to 62%. Queen size, growth rate, and soldier investment positively covaried. Only one model, the cost of replacement hypothesis (McKey 1979), correctly predicted that species with costly female alates invest more in defense. The two hypotheses linking apparency to defense may also be valid if fast-growing colonies are more likely to attract the attention of predators. 相似文献
14.
Marianne Elias Rainer Rosengren Liselotte Sundström 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2005,57(4):339-349
Ant colonies may have a single or several reproductive queens (monogyny and polygyny, respectively). In polygynous colonies, colony reproduction may occur by budding, forming multinest, polydomous colonies. In most cases, budding leads to strong genetic structuring within populations, and positive relatedness among nestmates. However, in a few cases, polydomous populations may be unicolonial, with no structuring and intra-nest relatedness approaching zero. We investigated the spatial organisation and genetic structure of a polygynous, polydomous population of Formica truncorum in Finland. F. truncorum shifts nest sites between hibernation and the reproductive season, which raises the following question: are colonies maintained as genetic entities throughout the seasons, or is the population unicolonial throughout the season? Using nest-specific marking and five microsatellite loci, we found a high degree of mixing between individuals of the population, and no evidence for a biologically significant genetic structuring. The nestmate relatedness was also indistinguishable from zero. Taken together, the results show that the population is unicolonial. In addition, we found that the population has undergone a recent bottleneck, suggesting that the entire population may have been founded by a very limited number of females. The precise causes for unicoloniality in this species remain open, but we discuss the potential influence of intra-specific competition, disintegration of recognition cues and the particular hibernation habits of this species. 相似文献
15.
Fungus gardening ants make clear choices among fungal substrates (food for their fungus). It has been proposed, but never
demonstrated, that these ants are collecting the best for their symbiotic fungus and the production of ant biomass (fitness).
The goal of this study was to determine whether preferred substrates lead to higher fitness in the attine, Trachymyrmex septentrionalis. Preferences exhibited by foragers were established. Colonies were fed a single substrate or a mixture of substrates during
the entire course of the experiment, which ended when sexual offspring appeared in the nest. The response variables were numbers
and weights of ant offspring and the chitin content of fungus gardens. Preference was not strongly related to fitness. The
preferred oak catkins produced the highest amounts of ant and fungal biomass, but the ants collected much more material than
needed, which indicates that forager activity is decoupled from fitness. The preferred caterpillar feces were rejected shortly
after the feedings began. The unpreferred oak leaves were just as effective at producing ant and fungal biomass as catkins.
Leaves are possibly unpreferred because they are expensive to cut. The unpreferred huckleberry flowers were inferior but did
not cause rejection behavior. The mixed diet was just as productive as catkins or leaves. This study indicates that foragers
possess a default mechanism to prefer catkins and frass, which can be quickly changed if substrates are bad. In contrast,
there does not appear to be a similar mechanism causing substrates to become preferred quickly. 相似文献
16.
Flavio Roces 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1993,33(3):183-189
Summary During recruitment, running velocity of both outbound and laden workers of the leaf-cutting ant Acromyrmex lundi depended on the information about resource quality they received from the first successful recruiter. In independent assays, single scout ants were allowed to collect sugar solutions of different concentrations and to recruit nestmates. Recruited workers were presented with standardized paper discs rather than the sugar solution given to the original recruiting ant. Outbound recruited workers were observed to run faster the more concentrated the solution found by the recruiter. Speed of disc-laden workers also depended on the concentration of the solution found by the recruiter, i.e. on the information about food quality they received, since they had no actual contact with the sugar solution. Disc-laden workers ran, as intuitively expected, slower than outbound workers. The reduction in speed, however, could not be attributed to the effects of the load itself, because workers collecting discs of the same weight, but with added sugar, ran as rapidly as outbound, unladen workers. Workers collecting standardized sugared discs reinforced the chemical trail on their way to the nest. The percentage of trail-layers was higher when workers were recruited to 10% than to 1% sugar solution, even though they collected the same kind of discs at the source. Their evaluation of resource quality, therefore, depended on their motivational state, which was modulated by the information they received during recruitment. Using previously published data on energetics of locomotion in leaf-cutting ants, travel costs of A. lundi workers recruited to sugar solutions of different concentration could be estimated. For workers recruited to the more concentrated solution, both speed and oxygen consumption rate increased by a roughly similar factor. Therefore, although workers ran faster to the high-quality resource, their actual energy investment per trip remained similar to that made by workers recruited to the low-quality resource. It is suggested that the more motivated workers reduced travel time without increasing energy costs during the trip. The adaptive value of these responses seems to be related to a rapid transmission of information about a newly discovered food source. 相似文献
17.
Summary. Leaf disc choice and oviposition bioassays were used to examine the effects of larval experience with a Hoodia gordonii latex on subsequent behaviors. The latex deterred feeding and oviposition by “naïve” cabbage looper (Trichoplusia ni, Noctuidae) larvae and moths with no previous exposure to the material. “Experienced” insects, reared on a diet with the H. gordonii latex (1000 ppm), exhibited lesser feeding deterrence relative to naïve insects. Experienced female moths actually preferred to lay eggs on treated rather than control leaves. There was no observed transfer of behavioral preferences from experienced parents to their offspring. Our results suggest that moths may be acquiring oviposition preferences from larval feeding experience as described by Hopkins’ host selection principal (HHSP) or through chemical legacy. 相似文献
18.
Marina Préault Olivier Chastel Frank Cézilly Bruno Faivre 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2005,58(5):497-505
In monogamous bird species, male parental investment may influence offspring fitness and females may gain advantages through mating with males providing extensive paternal care. However, paternal care is a benefit that can only be assessed indirectly because mate choice precedes paternal activities. Individual quality and age, both signalled by morphological characteristics, may reflect parental abilities. Because they may reflect individual foraging abilities, carotenoid-based colorations have been proposed to honestly signal parental quality. The blackbird (Turdus merula), a socially monogamous species, exhibits biparental care and males show bills that vary from pale yellow to orange due to carotenoid pigments. In this study, we investigated whether male bill colour and age are associated with parental ability. Our results suggest that males with more orange bills and older males are better fathers. Indeed, male visit rate increased with their bill colour index independently of age, and brood condition was higher for adult males, compared to yearlings, independently of bill colour. Overall, the number of fledglings produced was positively influenced by both the age of males and the colour intensity of their bills. Males with more orange bills and adults had a greater number of fledglings and these males also had higher levels of prolactin, a hormone known to promote parental care. This latter finding suggests that prolactin may be the link between carotenoid based colorations and the intensity of paternal effort. Thus, male bill colour seems to honestly reveal male physiological adjustment to paternal activities. 相似文献
19.
Summary. Larvae of the turnip sawfly, Athalia rosae L. (Hymenoptera: Tenthredinidae), sequester glucosinolates of their host plants, namely members of the Brassicaceae family,
in their haemolymph. Therefore, they need to circumvent myrosinase activities of the plant tissue which normally hydrolyse
the glucosinolates after plant damage. Effects of varying levels of glucosinolates and myrosinases on the performance of A. rosae were investigated using homozygous lines of Brassica juncea (L.) with either (1) low glucosinolate (lowGS) and low myrosinase (lowMR), (2) high glucosinolate (highGS) and high myrosinase (highMR), or (3) high glucosinolate (highGS) and low myrosinase (lowMR) levels. To insure that the given quantities remained as constant as possible, newly hatched larvae were enclosed on the
second-youngest leaf of a plant, and were offered a new plant of comparable physiological age (6-leaf-stage) every day. The
performance of A. rosae was little affected by leaf quality. Body masses of eonymphs and adults were on average lowest on the highGS/highMR-line, but these differences were rarely significant. The pupal developmental times of females and males were longest on
the highGS/lowMR-line in only one of two replicate experiments. All other performance traits (developmental times of larvae, egg numbers,
adult longevity) were not significantly different. Glucosinolates, sequestered by the larvae, are carried through the pupal
stage. The glucosinolate concentration measured in adult insects reflected the level of the host plant line, without showing
any obvious costs for sequestration. Obviously, A. rosae is highly tolerant to variation in the glucosinolate-myrosinase system of its host.
In addition, induced changes of glucosinolate concentrations and myrosinase activities caused by 24 h-feeding of groups of
three small larvae were analysed in the second-youngest leaves. In contrast to the patterns most herbivores evoke on Brassicaceae,
namely an increase of both glucosinolate concentration and myrosinase activity, we detected a significant decrease of both
traits in all three lines where the respective trait was originally high in the plants. Although glucosinolate levels dropped in the highGS lines about 50%, these still contained higher concentrations than the lowGS line. Whereas the activity of soluble myrosinases remained highest in the highMR line, even after a decrease to almost 30% due to feeding, the levels of insoluble myrosinases converged after feeding in
lowMR and highMR lines. Levels of the signalling molecule salicylic acid slightly decreased on average after feeding, whereas jasmonic acid
was below the detection threshold in almost all samples. The concentration of several molecules varies strongly in plant tissue
with age and can change due to induction by herbivore feeding. Therefore, if performance of an insect species is measured
on plants with specific traits, the variability in these traits needs to be carefully controlled in experiments. 相似文献
20.
Heikki Helanterä Stephen J. Martin Francis L. W. Ratnieks 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2007,62(2):223-228
We studied the effect of prior experience to eggs laid by nestmate and non-nestmate queens on the acceptance of queen-laid
eggs by worker wood ants, Formica fusca. We transferred eggs from a non-nestmate queen into colonies during early spring, when their own queen was recommencing egg
laying. A few weeks later, workers from these “experienced” colonies accepted eggs of both familiar (44% acceptance) and unfamiliar
(40%) non-nestmate queens much more than workers from control colonies (2%) that had only had previous contact with their
own queen’s eggs. Thus, prior exposure to eggs laid by a non-nestmate queen induces much greater acceptance of all non-nestmate
queen-laid eggs. Mechanistically, we hypothesize that exposure to eggs from several queens may increase acceptance by causing
a highly permissive acceptance threshold of non-nestmate queen-laid eggs rather than by widening the template for acceptable
queen-laid eggs. These novel results show that egg-discrimination behaviour in F. fusca is flexible and that workers respond to the diversity of eggs experienced in their colony. 相似文献