共查询到20条相似文献,搜索用时 11 毫秒
1.
Weapon size versus body size as a predictor of winning in fights between shore crabs, Carcinus maenas (L.) 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
Lynne U. Sneddon Felicity A. Huntingford Alan C. Taylor 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,41(4):237-242
Relative body size (carapace width) and weapon size (chela length) were used as indicators of resource holding potential (RHP)
in the agonistic behaviour of male shore crabs, Carcinus maenas (L.). Weapon size was found to be a more reliable predictor of the outcome of pairwise fights than body size. Crabs with
longer chelae than their opponents were more likely to win fights than crabs with relatively larger bodies. Body size had
less influence on the outcome of fights. Relative body and weapon size did not influence initiation of contests but did affect
the likelihood of winning; however, this was significant only for weapon size. Winning crabs had heavier claws with greater
surface area than losing crabs. There was no relationship between relative size and fight duration. The frequency of cheliped
display increased with chela length and win- ners performed significantly more displays than losers.
Received: 5 February 1997 / Accepted after revision: 20 May 1997 相似文献
2.
Multiple-queen (polygyne) colonies of the introduced fire ant Solenopsis invicta present a paradox for kin selection theory. Egg-laying queens within these societies are, on average, unrelated to one another,
and the numbers of queens per colony are high, so that workers appear to raise new sexuals that are no more closely related
to them than are random individuals in the population. This paradox could be resolved if workers discriminate between related
and unrelated nestmate sexuals in important fitness-related contexts. This study examines the possibility of such nepotism
using methods that combine the following features: (1) multiple relevant behavioral assays, (2) colonies with an unmanipulated
family structure, (3) multiple genetic markers with no known phenotypic effects, and (4) a statistical technique for distinguishing
between nepotism and potentially confounding phenomena. We estimated relatedness between interactants in polygyne S. invicta colonies in two situations, workers tending egg-laying queens and workers feeding maturing winged queens. In neither case
did we detect a significant positive value of relatedness that would implicate nepotism. We argue that the non-nepotistic
strategies displayed by these ants reflect historical selection pressures experienced by native populations, in which nestmate
queens are highly related to one another. The markedly different genetic structure in native populations may favor the operation
of stronger higher-level selection that effectively opposes weaker individual-level selection for nepotistic interactions
within nests.
Received: 28 June 1996 / Accepted after revision: 6 October 1996 相似文献
3.
Bertrand Schatz Jean-Paul Lachaud Guy Beugnon 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,40(6):337-349
According to the weight and size of their prey, Ectatomma ruidum workers can employ different recruitment systems (solitary hunting, cooperative hunting and group hunting with recruitment)
when mastering and retrieving prey items from short distances from the nest. Prey size determined the backwards entry typically
adopted by this species, while prey weight determined the predatory strategy selected. After a common initial sequence (search
for prey, detection, localization), predatory sequences varied in terms of the type of approach, the site of seizure, the
reaction after stinging and the type of transport. Nevertheless, irrespective of prey weight and size, seizure was preferentially
oriented towards the head and prey were always stung. Short-range recruitment and mass recruitment without trail laying were
elicited by a large range of heavy prey (> 2.5 times the weight of an individual worker). According to the mortality risk
associated with each prey, hunters exhibited a “prudent” stinging posture associated with an increase in the duration of the
subsequent phase of waiting for prey immobilization. The overall time of capture was positively correlated with the weight
of the prey. When collective hunting strategies were involved, E. ruidum colonies matched the number of recruited hunters to the size and weight of the prey. Compared to solitary hunting strategies,
for short food–nest distances, this graded recruitment appeared to enhance the energetic benefits derived by this species
from the use of recruitment systems: the higher the number of workers involved in the recruitment process, the greater the
energetic benefits obtained. The exhibition or absence of trail laying behavior in the recruitment responses displayed by
E. ruidum workers is discussed in relation to their involvement in scavenging or predatory behavior.
Received: 27 June 1996 / Accepted after revision: 3 March 1997 相似文献
4.
Sexual cohabitation as mate-guarding in the leaf-curling spider Phonognatha graeffei Keyserling (Araneoidea, Araneae) 总被引:1,自引:1,他引:1
The leaf-curling spider Phonognatha graeffei incorporates a twisted leaf into the central hub of its orb-web that is used as a retreat. This species is unusual among orb-weaving
spiders because males cohabit in the leaf retreat with both immature and mature females, mating with the former shortly after
the female molts. Cohabitation appears to be a form of mate-guarding because cohabiting males respond agonistically to rival
males that venture onto the web, and their behaviour depends upon the reproductive status of the female; males defending immature
females are more aggressive than those defending virgin, adult females. Males copulate with previously mated females for significantly
longer than with virgin females. Females may cannibalise cohabiting males, which occurs independently of whether the female
has been deprived of food. Females that cannibalise a single male do not have a higher fecundity than non-cannibalistic females.
Received: 2 February 1996 / Accepted after revision: 27 October 1996 相似文献
5.
The role of turbidity as a constraint on predator-prey interactions in aquatic environments 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
Many of the world's most productive aquatic ecosystems usually contain turbid water. Paradoxically, many fish species that
live in these habitats are also those that often rely on vision to detect their predators and their prey. For these fish,
turbidity will reduce the distance at which predator-prey interactions occur, and there should be a reduction in the opportunity
for behavioural modification to control the risk of predation. Under these conditions, we predict that most antipredator behaviour
will become ineffective and that predator-prey interactions in turbid water will be primarily characterised by the direct
effects of predator consumption of prey, rather than behavioural modification reducing the growth rates of prey. This hypothesis
was tested in two laboratory experiments. The first experiment investigated how water turbidity, risk of predation, and their
interaction affect habitat choice decisions by fathead minnows (Pimephales promelas). These data demonstrate that fathead minnows reduce their use of dangerous habitats, but that this effect is diminished
in turbid water. A second experiment examined mortality patterns when these fish were preyed upon by yellow perch (Perca flavescens) in clear and turbid water. Absolute mortality rates were unaffected by visibility, but patterns of mortality were random
in turbid water and skewed towards smaller individuals in clear water. Combined, these results support our hypothesis and
suggest that the impact of predation risk will be reduced in turbid aquatic ecosystems.
Received: 23 May 1996 / Accepted after revision: 18 November 1996 相似文献
6.
Common goldeneyes adjust maternal effort in relation to prior brood success and not current brood size 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
Parental investment theory predicts that parental effort should be related to the reproductive value of the current brood.
This depends on both the number of young and the survival prospects of each of them. Thus parents may provide more care to
larger broods either because of (1) the direct effect of brood size per se on reproductive value (the “brood size” hypothesis)
or because (2) past mortality, reflected in current brood size, predicts future mortality of the brood and hence its reproductive
value (the “brood success” hypothesis). Earlier studies have not attempted to distinguish between these alternatives. We tested
the hypotheses in the precocial, nidifugous common goldeneye Bucephala clangula, a species with uniparental female care. Maternal effort was measured as the time spent by the female in rearing the brood.
We found that brood size itself is not associated with maternal effort, but that females modify their maternal effort according
to the mortality already experienced by the brood, supporting the prediction of the brood success hypothesis. We also found
that brood mortality varied considerably between broods and that previous mortality predicts future mortality within broods,
basic assumptions of the brood success hypothesis.
Received: 30 January 1996 / Accepted after revision: 27 October 1996 相似文献
7.
Quality-indicating sexually selected traits may have their honesty maintained by their costs or by an inherent “revealing”
nature. Long tails in birds are usually considered to be costly “handicaps”, but may have additional potential as revealing
indicators through the incidence of breakage. Magpies Pica pica with unbroken and less abraded tails paired earlier, but did not nest or fledge young earlier than pairs with tails in poorer
condition. Pairs mated assortatively by tail quality, and magpies with very broken tails remained unmated. Pairs in which
both members had almost undamaged tails fledged more offspring than pairs with poorer tails. Tail quality did not correlate
with the extent of any habitat type in the territory. Tail damage thus honestly indicated a magpie's reproductive potential,
and the data are consistent with its having a role in mate choice, as a revealing element of tail morphology.
Received: 28 March 1996 / Accepted after revision: 9 December 1996 相似文献
8.
A molecular genetic analysis of social structure, dispersal, and interpack relationships of the African wild dog (Lycaon pictus ) 总被引:7,自引:0,他引:7
Derek J. Girman M. G. L. Mills Eli Geffen Robert K. Wayne 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,40(3):187-198
The African wild dog is a highly social, pack-living predator of the African woodland and savannah. The archetypal wild dog
pack consists of a single dominant breeding pair, their offspring, and non-breeding adults who are either offspring or siblings
of one of the breeding pair. Non-breeding adults cooperate in hunting, provisioning and the protection of young. From these
observations follows the prediction that the genetic structure of wild dogs packs should resemble that of a multigenerational
family, with all same-sexed adults and offspring within a pack related as sibs or half-sibs. Additionally, a higher kinship
between females from neighboring packs should be evident if females tend to have small dispersal distances relative to males.
We test these predictions through analysis of mitochondrial DNA control region sequences and 14 microsatellite loci in nine
wild dog packs from Kruger National Park, Republic of South Africa. We show that as predicted, African wild dog packs generally
consist of an unrelated alpha male and female, subdominant close relatives, and offspring of the breeding pair. Sub-dominant
wild dogs occasionally reproduce but their offspring rarely survive to 1 year of age. Relatedness influences the timing and
location of dispersal events as dispersal events frequently coincide with a change in pack dominance hierarchy and dispersers
often move to areas with a high proportion of close relatives.
Received: 22 February 1996 / Accepted after revision: 16 November 1996 相似文献
9.
J. E. Strassmann Carlos R. Solís C. R. Hughes Keith F. Goodnight David C. Queller 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,40(2):71-77
Colonies of social insects are sometimes viewed as superorganisms. The birth, reproduction, and death of colonies can be studied with demographic measures analogous to those normally applied to individuals, but two additional questions arise. First, how do adaptive colony demographies arise from individual behaviors? Second, since these superorganisms are made up of genetically distinct individuals, do conflicts within the colony sometimes modify and upset optima for colonies? The interplay between individual and superindividual or colony interests appears to be particularly complex in neotropical, swarm-founding, epiponine wasps such as Parachartergus colobopterus. In a long-term study of this species, we censused 286 nests to study colony-level reproduction and survivorship and evaluated individual-level factors by assessing genetic relatedness and queen production. Colony survivorship followed a negative exponential curve very closely, indicating type II survivorship. This pattern is defined by constant mortality across ages and is more characteristic of birds and other vertebrates than of insects. Individual colonies are long-lived, lasting an average of 347 days, with a maximum of over 4.5 years. The low and constant levels of colony mortality arise in part from colony initiation by swarming, nesting on protected substrates, and an unusual expandable nest structure. The ability to requeen rapidly was also important; relatedness data suggest that colonies requeen on average once every 9–12 months. We studied whether colony optima with respect to the timing of reproduction could be upset by individual worker interests. In this species, colonies are normally polygynous but new queens are produced only after a colony reaches the monogynous state, a result which is in accord with the genetic interests of workers. Therefore colony worker interests might drive colonies to reproduce whenever queen number happens to cycled down to one rather than at the season that is otherwise optimal. However, we found reproduction to be heavily concentrated in the rainy season. The number of new colonies peaked in this season as did the percentages of males and queens. Relatedness among workers reached a seasonal low of 0.21–0.27, reflecting the higher numbers of laying queens. This seasonality was achieved in part by a modest degree of synchrony in the queen reduction cycle. Worker relatedness reached peaks of around 0.4 in the dry season, reflecting a decrease to a harmonic mean queen number of about 2.5. Thus, a significant number of colonies must be approaching monogyny entering the rainy season. Coupled with polygynous colonies rearing only males (split sex ratios), this makes it possible for a colony cycle driven by selfish worker interests to be consistent with concentrating colony reproduction during a favorable season. 相似文献
10.
Control of reproduction in social insect colonies: individual and collective relatedness preferences in the paper wasp, Polistes annularis 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
David C. Queller J. M. Peters Carlos R. Solís Joan E. Strassmann 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,40(1):3-16
Social insect colonies often have one or a few queens. How these queens maintain their reproductive monopoly, when other
colony members could gain by sharing in the reproduction, is not generally known. DNA microsatellite genotyping is used to
determine reproductive interests of various classes of colony members in the paper wasp, Polistes annularis. The relatedness estimates show that the best outcome for most individuals is to be the reproductive egg-layer. For workers,
this depends on the sex of offspring: they should prefer to lay their own male eggs, but are indifferent if the queen lays
the female eggs. The next-best choice is usually to support the current queen. As a rule, subordinates and workers should
prefer the current queen to reproduce over other candidates (though subordinates have no strong preference for the queen over
other subordinates, and workers may prefer other workers as a source of male eggs). This result supports the theory that reproductive
monopoly stems from the collective preferences of non-reproductives, who suppress each other in favor of the queen. However,
we reject the general hypothesis of collective worker control in this species because its predictions about who should succeed
after the death of the present queen are not upheld. The first successor is a subordinate foundress even though workers should
generally prefer a worker successor. If all foundresses have died, an older worker succeeds as queen, in spite of a collective
worker preference for a young worker. The results support the previous suggestion that age serves as a conventional cue serving
to reduce conflict over queen succession.
Received: 3 May 1996 / Accepted after revision: 22 September 1996 相似文献
11.
Janine R. Clemmons 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,40(4):243-251
In a variety of avian species, vocalizations used to stimulate gaping in nestlings during feeding share similar features
such as broad frequency range, multiple spectral components, and sudden onset. It is currently not understood what functional
significance, if any, these acoustic features have for close-range communication. One example of such a vocalization is the
“squawk” of the black-capped chickadees (Parus atricapillus). The present study analyzes the significant features of the squawk by observing the response of chickadee nestlings to field
playbacks of natural and computer-modified squawks. Of the features tested, frequency range appears most critical, but responses
to frequency ranges change with age, such that low frequencies are most stimulating around the time of hatching but cease
to elicit gaping after 5 days posthatch. Aperiodic stimuli and computer-modified squawks with gradual onsets tend to be less
stimulating than natural squawks at some ages, but these differences are not significant at any age. The functional significance
of the broad frequency range of the squawk is discussed, including the possible role of accommodating shifting frequency preferences
related to ontogenetic changes in auditory sensitivity and elaboration of the vocal repertoire.
Received: 16 January 1996 / Accepted after revision: 28 December 1996 相似文献
12.
Relatedness, polyandry and extra-group paternity in the cooperatively-breeding white-browed scrubwren (Sericornis frontalis ) 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
Linda A. Whittingham Peter O. Dunn Robert D. Magrath 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,40(4):261-270
We used DNA fingerprinting to examine the genetic parentage and mating system of the cooperatively breeding white-browed
scrubwren, Sericornis frontalis, in Canberra, Australia. Our analyses revealed a remarkable variety of mating tactics and social organization. Scrubwrens
bred in pairs or multi-male groups that consisted of a female and two or more males. Females were always unrelated to the
pair male or alpha (dominant) male. Among multi-male groups we found three different mating tactics. Firstly, when alpha and
beta (subordinate) males were unrelated, they usually shared paternity in the brood. This resulted in both males gaining reproductive
benefits directly. Secondly, when beta males were not related to the female but were related to the alpha males, beta males
sired offspring in some broods. In this situation, beta males gained reproductive benefits both directly and potentially indirectly
(through the related alpha male). Thirdly, when beta males were related to the female or both the female and alpha male, they
remained on their natal territory and did not sire any offspring. Thus beta males gained only indirect reproductive benefits.
Overall, when group members were related closely, the dominant male monopolized reproductive success, whereas when the members
were not related closely the two males shared paternity equally. This positive association between monopolization of reproduction
and relatedness is predicted by models of reproductive skew, but has not been reported previously within a single population
of birds. Other cooperatively breeding birds with both closely related and unrelated helpers may show a similar variety of
mating tactics. Finally, we found that extra-group paternity was more common in pairs (24% of young) than in multi-male groups
(6%), and we discuss three possible reasons for this difference.
Received: 21 May 1996 / Accepted after revision: 14 December 1996 相似文献
13.
Testosterone stress does not increase asymmetry of a hormonally mediated sexual ornament in a lizard
José P. Veiga Alfredo Salvador Pilar López José Martin 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,41(3):171-176
The theory that fluctuating asymmetry is sensitive to both environmental and genetic stress is gaining acceptance among evolutionary
biologists. Most empirical work has focused on ornamental traits on the assumption that they are more susceptible to stress.
In tegumentary coloration is a common ornament in nature, and frequently has a hormonal basis. Earlier studies in the lizard
Psammodromus algirus indicate that testosterone induces the development of head nuptial coloration in large males and, at the same time, produces
an increase of the ectoparasite load and higher mortality. Hence, the manipulation of testosterone levels may be a way to
increase ornament expression and simultaneously create conditions that may make symmetrical development difficult. This positive
covariation between character size and symmetry is opposite to that expected in theory for sexually selected traits, so the
predicted elevation of asymmetry due to the treatment cannot be confounded by any intrinsic association between symmetry and
character size. We firstly consider the effect that testosterone supplementation has on two variables that reflect the symmetry
of bilateral throat nuptial coloration in large P. algirus males. Also, we examine whether ornament symmetry is positively associated with reproductive success, a prediction of theory
of sexually selected symmetry. Testosterone treatment did not increase the fluctuating asymmetry of throat coloration. Size
asymmetry increased with character size in individuals with a fragmented colour pattern, but changed suddenly to a highly
symmetric pattern in individuals with non-fragmented coloration. Mirror asymmetry decreased steadily with character size.
These results suggest that the development of coloration on both sides of the throat midline follows a random pattern. Asymmetry
did not correlate with variables that estimated reproductive success, suggesting that asymmetry is not affected by physiological
stress and that this trait is not a sexually selected signal in P. algirus.
Received: 1 July 1996 / Accepted after revision: 4 May 1997 相似文献
14.
Ellen J. Censky 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,40(4):221-225
Mate choice by females has been documented in a variety of taxa. Female mate choice in species lacking male resource control
or paternal care might occur if preferred males provide protection from harassment. Female mate choice was investigated in
a natural population of the non-territorial lizard Ameiva plei (Teiidae). Consort pairs were allowed to form naturally. Consort males were significantly larger than non-consort males.
After removal of consort males, the “abandoned” female's reaction to the first male who approached her was recorded. Females
rejected all small males. Female preference for large males was significantly higher than preference for small males. Large
males may be better equipped to guard the females from harassment and behavior of large males is less harassing than behavior
of small males, thereby affording the female increased foraging time.
Received: 21 June 1996 / Accepted after revision: 28 December 1996 相似文献
15.
P. Jivoff 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,40(3):175-185
In many species, post-copulatory mate guarding prevents other males from mating with the guarded female. In crabs, males
stay with their mates to protect the female from predators because, in some species, mating occurs when she is soft and vulnerable
after molting. I tested the relative roles of sperm competition and predation on the duration of the post-copulatory association
in the blue crab, Callinectes sapidus. Unpaired females suffered greater predation mortality than paired females and males stayed with the female longer in the
presence of predators than in their absence, suggesting that the post-copulatory association protects females during their
vulnerable period. However, the association may also occur in blue crabs because of sperm competition since spermathecal contents
of females in the field indicate that 12.4% mated twice. Females experimentally mated with two males contained both males
ejaculates and each ejaculate had access to the unfertilized eggs, suggesting that the size of a male's ejaculate influences
his fertilization rate in a multiply-mated female. Males stayed longest in response to a high risk of sperm competition. Longer
post-copulatory associations allowed the first male's ejaculate to harden into a type of sperm plug, which limited the size
of a second inseminator's ejaculate in a non-virgin female as compared with a virgin. Males passed larger ejaculates in the
presence of rivals and when previous ejaculates were in the female spermathecae, another response to sperm competition. Larger
ejaculates may need longer post-copulatory associations before a more effective sperm plug forms. Large males stayed with
the female longer, which is consistent with their ability to pass larger ejaculates than small males and suggests that there
may be costs to minimizing the duration of the post-copulatory association. In the field, associations last long enough to
protect the female during her vulnerable phase and may ensure that the guarding male fertilizes the most eggs in the female,
even if she remates. Thus, the post-copulatory association protects female blue crabs from additional inseminators as well
as from predators.
Received: 23 January 1996 / Accepted after revision: 9 November 1996 相似文献
16.
Codas, which are patterned series of clicks, were recorded from female and immature sperm whales (Physeter macrocephalus) in a number of locations around the South Pacific Ocean and in the Caribbean Sea. Using K-means cluster analysis, 3,644 codas were categorized based on the number of clicks and their patterning. There were 30 resulting
types of coda. The numbers of codas of the different types recorded were used to construct repertoires for each recording
session, day, group of whales, place, area, and ocean. Strong group-specific dialects, which seem to persist over periods
of years, were apparent, overlaid on weaker geographical variation. Significant differences in repertoire were found between
the Caribbean and the Pacific Ocean. Sperm whales now join killer whales (Orcinus orca) as the only cetacean species in which dialects (differences in vocal repertoire among neighboring, potentially interacting
groups) have been found.
Received: 13 June 1996 / Accepted after revision: 19 January 1997 相似文献
17.
José L. Tella Manuela G. Forero José A. Donázar Juan J. Negro Fernando Hiraldo 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,40(4):253-260
We examined the causes, costs and benefits of adoption in the altricial lesser kestrel Falco naumanni. Specifically, we tested the intergenerational conflict hypothesis, proposed to explain adoption in some birds. Adoptions
involved 76% of the nests and 51% of the nestlings at a mean age of 25 days (12 days before fledging). Nest-switching nestlings
were not in poorer body condition, more parasitized or younger than their siblings, and body condition and prey delivery rates
of their parents did not differ from those of other parents. In the foster nest, adopted nestlings did not benefit from higher
feeding rates or a prolongation of the nestling period. They did not have fewer nest-mates or achieve higher rank within the
new brood. Thus, adopted nestlings did not improve their body condition and survival. Adult lesser kestrels seemed unable
to finely discriminate beween their own and alien chicks. Foster parents bore the cost of an increase of prey delivery rates,
although it did not affect their survival or subsequent reproductive performance. Therefore, our results do not support the
intergenerational conflict hypothesis, and suggest that adoption in this species is non-adaptive. Traditionally, the lesser
kestrel bred in cliffs where movement among nest-sites was restricted. Nowadays, about half of the colonies are in tiled roofs
which facilitate nest-switching by nestlings. The high rate of adoptions may thus be explained as reproductive errors associated
with the recent occupation of a new breeding habitat.
Received: 3 May 1996 / Accepted after revision: 19 January 1997 相似文献
18.
Juan Carlos Alonso Luis M. Bautista Javier A. Alonso 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,40(6):401-408
We studied the behavior of 13 radiotagged cranes dispersing from a communal roost over days when they changed their main
daily foraging area between consecutive days during two winter seasons. Individuals went to a new foraging zone when on the
previous day their morning food intake had fallen below their mean morning food intake measured over the whole winter. Food
intake on the day before a change in foraging area was positively correlated with dominance rank. Dominant cranes changed
to new zones with higher numbers of birds and food density, while subordinate cranes went to new zones with lower numbers
of birds. As a result, all birds increased their food intake over that of the previous day. Dominant cranes remained more
faithful to their most preferred foraging zone, where they spent 69% of the mornings, while subordinate birds were more mobile,
switching among zones frequently. Dominant birds left the roost later than subordinate birds on the days they changed to a
new zone, which could be used to track the main departing flows. The results suggest that the dynamics that led to a truncated
phenotype-limited distribution were determined by social dominance and food abundance, with dominant cranes shifting to a
new zone to maintain their high intake levels and subordinates changing more frequently whenever their daily intake did not
reach the minimum metabolic requirements.
Received: 16 December 1996 / Accepted after revision: 22 February 1997 相似文献
19.
Reproductive dominance and differential ovicide in the communally breeding burying beetle Nicrophorus tomentosus 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
Michelle Pellissier Scott 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,40(5):313-320
Communal breeding can be characterized by the division of reproduction among cooperating individuals and, if the distribution
of reproduction is inequitable, by the mechanisms for achieving skewed reproductive success. The burying beetle (Nicrophorus tomentosus) is a facultative communal breeder. Unrelated adults, especially females, provide extensive parental care to broods of mixed
parentage. The frequency and degree of reproductive skew between two females were examined experimentally. On medium-size
carcasses, the proportion of eggs attributed to each female was not significantly different from random in 42% of the broods,
skewed in 42% and not shared in 16%. Although reproduction was usually skewed in favor of the larger female, the relative
sizes of the two females did not predict the degree of skew. On large carcasses, the proportion of eggs attributed to each
female was not different from random in 87% of the broods and weakly skewed in 13%. Several mechanisms for biasing reproductive
success were investigated. Females increase the proportion of their offspring in the brood by committing differential ovicide.
Secondly, burying and preparing a carcass cooperatively stimulates ovarian development of the larger female and slows it for
the smaller female, reducing or delaying oviposition by the subordinate. Thirdly, larger females are more likely to be dominant
and are more fecund than smaller females.
Received: 20 July 1996 / Accepted after revision: 30 November 1996 相似文献
20.
Russell C. Titus C. Ray Chandler Ellen D. Ketterson Val Nolan Jr. 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,41(3):165-169
Frequency of singing by birds may vary with reproductive stage in ways that reflect variation in the functions of song in
intersexual and intrasexual communication. In dark-eyed juncos (Junco hyemalis) high-amplitude song is produced only by males. To investigate the function of this song, we tested whether fertility of
females affected singing by their mates or by neighboring males. Using focal observations, song censuses, and radiotracking
data, we determined whether song production varied between and among periods when females were fertile and non-fertile. Our
findings show that males do not increase song production when their mates are fertile, nor do they increase song production
when neighboring females are fertile. These results suggest that male juncos do not signal their intent to defend territories
(or mates) more when females are fertile and that they do not use song to advertise to specific potential participants in
extra-pair fertilizations.
Received: 13 February 1997 / Accepted after revison: 2 May 1997 相似文献