共查询到5条相似文献,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
T. D. Seeley Susanne Kühnholz Anja Weidenmüller 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1996,39(6):419-427
If a forager bee returns to her hive laden with high-quality nectar but then experiences difficulty finding a receiver bee
to unload her, she will begin to produce a conspicuous communication signal called the tremble dance. The context in which
this signal is produced suggests that it serves to stimulate more bees to function as nectar receivers, but so far there is
no direct evidence of this effect. We now report an experiment which shows that more bees do begin to function as nectar receivers
when foragers produce tremble dances. When we stimulated the production of tremble dances in a colony and counted the number
of bees engaged in nectar reception before and after the period of intense tremble dancing, we found a dramatic increase.
In two trials, the number of nectar receivers rose from 17% of the colony’s population before tremble dancing to 30–50% of
the population after the dancing. We also investigated which bees become the additional nectar receivers, by looking at the
age composition of the receiver bees before and after the period of intense tremble dancing. We found that none of the bees
recruited to the task of nectar reception were old bees, most were middle-aged bees, and some were even young bees. It remains
unclear whether these auxiliary nectar receivers were previously inactive (as a reserve supply of labor) or were previously
active on other tasks. Overall, this study demonstrates that a honey bee colony is able to rapidly and strongly alter its
allocation of labor to adapt to environmental changes, and it further documents one of the communication mechanisms underlying
this ability.
Received: 31 May 1996/Accepted after revision: 9 August 1996 相似文献
2.
During house hunting, honeybee, Apis melli- fera, workers perform the vibration signal, which may function in a modulatory manner to influence several aspects of nestsite
selection and colony movement. We examined the role of the vibration signal in the house-hunting process of seven honeybee
swarms. The signal was performed by a small proportion of the older bees, and 20% of the vibrating bees also performed waggle
dances for nestsites. Compared to non-vibrating controls, vibrating bees exhibited increased rates of locomotion, were more
likely to move into the interiors of the swarms, and were more likely to fly from the clusters and perform waggle dances.
Recipients responded to the signal with increased locomotion and were more likely than non- vibrated controls to fly from
the swarms. Because vibration signals were intermixed with waggle dances by some vibrators, and because they stimulated flight
in recipients, the signals may have enhanced nestsite scouting and recruitment early in the house-hunting process. All swarms
exhibited increased vibration activity within 0.5–1 h of departure. During these final periods, numerous vibrating bees wove
repeatedly in and out of the clusters while signaling and motion on the swarms increased until it culminated in mass flight.
The peaks of vibration activity observed at the end of the house-hunting process may therefore have activated the entire swarm
for liftoff once a new nestsite had been selected. Thus, the vibration signal may help to integrate the behavior of numerous
groups of workers during nestsite selection and colony relocation.
Received: 17 January 2000 / Received in revised form: 5 April 2000 / Accepted: 3 May 2000 相似文献
3.
Tremble dances are sometimes performed by returning forager bees instead of waggle dances. Recent studies by Seeley (1992) and Kirchner (1993) have revealed that this behaviour is part of the recruitment communication system of bees. The ultimate cause of tremble dances is, according to Seeley (1992), an imbalance between the nectar intake rate and the nectar processing capacity of the colony. This imbalance is correlated with a long initial search time of returning foragers to find bees to unload them. However, it remained unclear whether a long search time is the direct proximate cause of tremble dancing. Here we report that a variety of experimental conditions can elicit tremble dances. All of them have in common that the total search time that foragers spend searching for unloaders, until they are fully unloaded, is prolonged. This finding supports and extends the hypothesis that a long search time is the proximate cause of tremble dancing. The results also confirm the previous reports of Lindauer (1948) and others about factors eliciting tremble dancing. 相似文献
4.
The regulation of pollen foraging by honey bees: how foragers assess the colony's need for pollen 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Scott Camazine 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1993,32(4):265-272
Summary The honey bee colony presents a challenging paradox. Like an organism, it functions as a coherent unit, carefully regulating its internal milieu. But the colony consists of thousands of loosely assembled individuals each functioning rather autonomously. How, then, does the colony acquire the necessary information to organize its work force? And how do individuals acquire information about specific colony needs, and thus know what tasks need be performed? I address these questions through experiments that analyze how honey bees acquire information about the colony's need for pollen and how they regulate its collection. The results demonstrate features of the colony's system for regulating pollen foraging: (1) Pollen foragers quickly acquire new information about the colony's need for pollen. (2) When colony pollen stores are supplemented, many pollen foragers respond by switching to nectar foraging or by remaining in the hive and ceasing to forage at all. (3) Pollen foragers do not need direct contact with pollen to sense the colony's change of state, nor do they use the odor of pollen as a cue to assess the colony's need for pollen. (4) Pollen foragers appear to obtain their information about colony pollen need indirectly from other bees in the hive. (5) The information takes the form of an inhibitory cue. The proposed mechanism for the regulation of pollen foraging involves a hierarchical system of information acquisition and a negative feedback loop. By taking advantage of the vast processing capacity of large numbers of individuals working in parallel, such a system of information acquisition and dissemination may be ideally suited to promote efficient regulation of labor within the colony. Although each individual relies on only limited, local information, the colony as a whole achieves a finely-tuned response to the changing conditions it experiences. 相似文献
5.
Vicarious sampling: the use of personal and public information by starlings foraging in a simple patchy environment 总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6
Group foragers may be able to assess patch quality more efficiently by paying attention to the sampling activities of conspecifics
foraging in the same patch. In a previous field experiment, we showed that starlings foraging on patches of hidden food could
use the successful foraging activities of others to help them assess patch quality. In order to determine whether a starling
could also use another individual’s lack of foraging success to assess and depart from empty patches more quickly, we carried
out two experimental studies which compared the behaviour of captive starlings sampling artificial patches both when alone
and when in pairs. Solitary starlings were first trained to assess patch quality in our experimental two-patch system, and
were then tested on an empty patch both alone and with two types of partner bird. One partner sampled very few holes and thus
provided a low amount of public information; the other sampled numerous holes and thus provided a high amount of public information.
In experiment 1, we found no evidence of vicarious sampling. Subjects sampled a similar number of empty holes when alone as
when with the low and high information partners; thus they continued to rely on their own personal information to make their
patch departure decisions. In experiment 2, we modified the experimental patches, increasing the ease with which a bird could
watch another’s sampling activities, and increasing the difficulty of acquiring accurate personal sampling information. This
time, subjects apparently did use public information, sampling fewer empty holes before departure when with the high-information
partner than when with the low-information partner, and sampling fewer holes when with the low-information partner than when
alone. We suggest that the degree to which personal and public information are used is likely to depend both on a forager’s
ability to remember where it has already sampled and on the type of environment in which foraging takes place.
Received: 31 January 1995/Accepted after revision: 11 September 1995 相似文献