共查询到14条相似文献,搜索用时 15 毫秒
1.
A. P. Moller F. de Lope J. M. López Caballero 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1995,37(5):289-295
Secondary sexual characters are assumed to be costly to produce or maintain. A test of this assumption was performed using the sexually exaggerated outermost tail feathers of male barn swallows Hirundo rustica, a trait currently subject of a directional female mate preference. A possible cost of sexual signalling in male barn swallows arises from increased flight cost during foraging in this aerially insectivorous species. A longer tail may impose a greater drag during flight and thereby affect foraging ability. This was tested by determining the relationship between experimentally modified male tail lengths and number and size of prey delivered to offspring in Spain, where sexual size dimorphism in tail length is small, compared to Denmark, where dimorphism is large. Food boluses contained significantly fewer small insects in Spain than in Denmark. Males with elongated tails captured more and smaller insects while males with shortened tails captured fewer and larger prey items at both sites. Males with naturally long tails were less affected by experimental treatment in terms of effects on the number and the size of prey delivered to their offspring, a finding consistent with a long tail being a condition-dependent viability indicator. The effect of a given degree of tail manipulation on prey size and number of prey per bolus was larger in Spain than in Denmark. These results demonstrate that (1) tail length in male barn swallows affects foraging, and (2) larger sexual size dimorphism occurs where the foraging cost of an increment in ornament size is smallest.Communicated by M. Zuk 相似文献
2.
Testosterone-induced depression of male parental behavior in the barn swallow: female compensation and effects on seasonal fitness 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
In birds, many aspects of male socio-sexual and parental behavior are influenced by androgens, most notably testosterone (T). We report the effects of subcutaneous T-implants in male barn swallows (Hirundo rustica) on male and female parental behavior and on seasonal reproductive success. Males were assigned to one of three experimental groups: (i) implanted with a T-filled Silastic tube; (ii) implanted with an empty Silastic tube; and (iii) not implanted. T-implanted males provided a smaller proportion of feedings (number of feedings by the male/total number of feedings by both parents) and fed nestlings less frequently (number of feedings/h) than males of the other two groups. Females paired to T-implanted males fed nestlings significantly more often than females paired with unimplanted males. Females almost fully compensated for their mates' shortfall, and this resulted in similar combined feeding efforts among treatments. Reproductive success in their first broods or during the entire breeding season was unaffected by T- treatment. These results confirm earlier reports of the suppressive effects of T on male parental behavior. However, they are inconsistent with current ESS models that predict partial compensation as the optimal response by one individual to reduction of parental effort by its mate in monogamous, biparental systems. 相似文献
3.
Oscar Vedder Arjan L. Dekker G. Henk Visser Cor Dijkstra 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2005,58(4):429-436
Allocation of parental investment is predicted to be equal at the population level between both sexes of offspring, and should lead to sex ratio biases in species that exhibit a sex-difference in parental care. Sex-differences in parental care are rarely quantified. We measured daily energy expenditure in free-living nestlings of the extremely sexually size dimorphic European sparrowhawk (Accipiter nisus), using the doubly labelled water method. These data were combined with measured growth characteristics to estimate daily and total metabolised energy intake of male and female young during the nestling stage. Females reached an asymptotic body mass 1.6 times higher than males. This resulted in a total metabolised energy an estimated 1.4 times higher for the nestling stage. Furthermore, we observed a decline in daily metabolised energy with an increase in brood size, which was significantly stronger for females than for males. These results are discussed in the context of Fishers equal allocation theory. Empirical evidence of a sex ratio bias at the end of parental care, with an overall excess of males, is lacking in this species. Consequently, our data do not support the idea of equal allocation between the sexes. The observed sex difference in daily metabolised energy in response to brood size may give scope for sex ratio bias at the level of the individual brood. 相似文献
4.
Anders Pape Møller 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2007,61(8):1311-1319
Protandry reflects the earlier arrival of males than females to the site of reproduction. Such protandry is hypothesised to
arise from sex differences in costs and benefits of early arrival. I investigated temporal patterns of arrival date of male
and female barn swallows Hirundo rustica and temporal patterns of selection to test the hypothesis that sex differences in selection account for sex differences in
arrival date. Mean arrival date of male barn swallows but not of females advanced during the last 33 years, giving rise to
an increasing sex difference in arrival date. Early arrival was favoured by increasingly better survival in males, while females
showed an opposite pattern that did not reach significance, although the effect differed between sexes. Early arrival increased
fecundity in both sexes equally.The sex difference in viability selection in relation to arrival date changed from positive
to negative as the degree of protandry increased in recent years, although there was no similar significant relationship for
fecundity selection. Furthermore, sex differences in viability selection in a given year affected the degree of protandry
in the following year through differential survival of certain phenotypes over others. Finally, temporal changes in sex difference
in viability selection and protandry were related to an increase in the interval between first and second clutches, as the
duration of the breeding season increased because of climatic amelioration. These findings suggest that arrival date is under
divergent selection in the two sexes, providing a mechanism for the evolution of protandry. 相似文献
5.
Oddmund Kleven Frode Jacobsen Rasa Izadnegahdar Raleigh J. Robertson Jan T. Lifjeld 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2006,59(3):412-418
In socially monogamous species, extra-pair paternity has the potential to increase the variance in male reproductive success,
thereby affecting the opportunity for sexual selection on male extravagant ornamentation. In the European barn swallow (Hirundo rustica rustica), the tail streamer length is a sexually selected male ornament and an honest indicator of viability. The North American
barn swallow (Hirundo rustica erythrogaster) also shows sexual dimorphism in tail streamer length, but whether this trait holds the same signalling function in this
subspecies is a controversial issue, and the available literature is presently scarce. Here, we present data on paternity
in the North American barn swallow, including a complete sampling of extra-pair sires in four colonies. We analysed how extra-pair
paternity affected the variance in male fertilization success and examined whether male tail streamer (i.e. the outermost
tail feather) length correlated with fertilization success (n=86 males). Extra-pair paternity constituted 31% of all offspring and significantly increased the variance in male fertilization
success. The number of offspring sired by extra-pair males accounted for almost half of the total variance in male fertilization
success. Males with naturally long tail streamers had a higher fertilization success than males with shorter tail streamers,
and this pattern was mainly caused by a higher extra-pair success for males with long tail streamers. Males with long tail
streamers also paired with early breeding females in prime body condition. These results are consistent with the idea that
there is directional sexual selection on male tail streamer length, possibly mediated through male extra-pair mating success
or the timing of breeding onset. 相似文献
6.
Behavioural research on domestic pigs has included parent–offspring conflict, sibling competition, and the use of signals
which influence resource allocation. In this paper, we review key sow–piglet behavioural studies and discuss their relevance
to resource allocation theory. Sibling competition begins in the uterus and continues after birth, as piglets compete directly
for access to the sow’s teats. This competition is made more severe by a unique dentition, which newborn piglets use to lacerate
the faces of siblings during teat disputes. Competition often leads to the death of some littermates, especially those of
low birth weight. Piglets also compete indirectly for milk, apparently by stimulating milk production at the teats that they
habitually use at the expense of milk production by other teats. The complex nursing behavior of the sow appears designed
to prevent the more vigorous piglets from monopolizing the milk. Sows give vocal signals which both attract piglets to suckle
and synchronize their behavior during nursing episodes. Piglets give loud vocal signals when separated from the sow; calls
which vary in intensity and appear to be honest signals of need. Udder massage by piglets may also serve as an honest signal
of need. Parent–offspring conflict has been demonstrated experimentally in pigs. Specifically, when given the opportunity
to control contact with their piglets, sows nurse less frequently, provide less milk, and lose less weight during lactation
than sows that cannot control their level of contact. Because of this interesting natural history and because they are so
amenable to experimentation, domestic pigs provide a rich system for testing ideas drawn from resource allocation theory.
This contribution is part of the special issue “Sibling competition and cooperation in mammals” (guest editors: Robyn Hudson
and Fritz Trillmich). 相似文献
7.
Parent–offspring conflict theory is well supported by theoretical arguments. However, empirical observations are often difficult to interpret and have contradicted one of its most appealing predictions that parent and offspring should disagree over killing of nest or littermates. We present the first examples of deadly conflict between siblings of different cohorts. In Galápagos fur seals (Arctocephalus galapagoensis) and sea lions (Zalophus wollebaeki), mothers often wean their single offspring at 2 years. This leads to a situation where up to 23% of all pups are born while the older sibling is still being nursed. Younger siblings are disadvantaged by being born lighter than neonates without older still dependent siblings. Pups born while an older sib is still dependent grow less in early life (fur seal) and suffer increased early mortality (both species) through direct aggression or scramble competition with the older sibling. This effect is much stronger in years of high sea surface temperature (El Niño) indicating low marine productivity and if the older offspring is a male. In both species, mothers interfere aggressively in this conflict by defending the younger offspring. In years of El Niño, intense resistance to maternal aggression by the older offspring happens frequently in the fur seal. Such resistance against weaning can induce maternal neglect of the newborn. Given substantial year to year variation in offspring growth, maternal aggression forces weaning in the older sibling only if it has reached sufficient size to support itself by foraging. In Galápagos fur seals, pups with older siblings can either represent insurance against loss of older offspring or extra reproductive value. 相似文献
8.
Offspring should be selected to influence maternal effort to maximize their own fitness, whereas mothers are selected to limit
investment in present progeny. In mammals, this leads to a conflict over the amount of milk provided and the timing of weaning.
The intensity and time course of such conflict has so far mostly been investigated experimentally in altricial rodents. However,
it is expected that offspring options for conflict will depend on developmental state. We therefore investigated in the highly
precocial domestic guinea pig (Cavia aperea f. porcellus) who decides over nursing performance and weaning and how pup state influences these decisions. Specifically, we tested whether
a threshold mass of pups predicts weaning time. By exchanging older litters against neonates and vice versa, we produced a
situation in which females differed in lactational stage from the cross-fostered pups. Our results indicate that females decide
about the timing of weaning, as cross-fostered younger pups were weaned at a much younger age than controls and older pups
benefited from continuing lactation of foster mothers. Growth rates did not differ in the treatment groups, and different
weaning ages resulted in differing weaning mass refuting the hypothesis that weaning is based on a threshold mass of offspring.
This constitutes clear evidence that in a precocial rodent, the guinea pig, decisions about maternal care are primarily determined
by maternal state and little influenced by pup state despite the extreme precociality of offspring. We suggest that precocial
pups show little resistance to early weaning when food is abundant, as they reach sufficient nutritional independence by the
middle of lactation to enable independent survival. 相似文献
9.
The theory of sex allocation suggests that if the reproductive value and the cost of producing/rearing offspring differ between
male and female offspring, parents should invest differently in sexes depending on environmental conditions. Female parents
could allocate more resources to eggs of one sex to compensate potential sex-dependent constraints later during the nestling
period. In this study, we tested the influence of environmental conditions on sexual dimorphism in eggs of Eurasian kestrels
(Falco tinnunculus) by experimentally manipulating food availability before laying. We found that an increase in food abundance before laying
did not increase egg mass but changed sex-dependent resource distribution in eggs. In food-supplemented pairs, but not in
control pairs, egg mass and hatchling mass were similar between males and females. In addition, we found, in the food-supplemented
group, that the latest hatched females showed shorter hatching times than in the control group. In control pairs, female eggs,
hatchlings and nestlings were heavier than males. In addition, male fledglings in the food-supplemented group gained less
mass than those in the control group. As that food abundance was only increased until the onset of laying, female kestrels
were expected to invest in eggs taking food abundance before egg formation as a predictor of future conditions during brood
rearing. Our study shows that environmental conditions before laying promote a subtle adjustment of the resources invested
in both sexes of offspring rather than in other breeding parameters. This adjustment resulted in a shortening of hatching
time of the last hatched females that possibly gives them advantages in their competitive capacity with respect to male nest-mates. 相似文献
10.
Sexual selection that results in the evolution of exaggerated secondary sexual characters has been hypothesized to impose production and maintenance costs of such traits on their bearers. Costs arising from sexual selection could increase the intensity of predator-mediated natural selection, leading to the prediction that species with exaggerated secondary sexual characters should be particularly susceptible to predation. We tested this prediction in a comparative analysis based on 31,745 prey individuals belonging to 66 species of birds collected from a total of 937 breeding events by 33 to 66 different pairs of European sparrowhawks Accipiter nisus annually during a period of 21 years. To assess vulnerability of different species we estimated a prey vulnerability index based on the difference in the logarithmically transformed absolute abundance of prey minus the logarithmically transformed expected abundance as determined by population density of breeding birds. The prey vulnerability index was predicted by sexual dichromatism, accounting for 23% of the variance in risk of predation among species, even when considering similarity in phenotype among species due to common descent (in the latter case explaining 12% of the variance). This finding suggests that sexual selection is an important evolutionary force-affecting predator–prey interactions.Electronic Supplementary Material Supplementary material is available for this article at 相似文献
11.
Carlos César Martínez-Rivera H. Carl Gerhardt 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2008,63(2):195-208
Senders and receivers influence dynamic characteristics of the signals used for mate attraction over different time scales.
On a moment-to-moment basis, interactions among senders competing for a mate influence dynamic characteristics, whereas the
preferences of receivers of the opposite gender exert an influence over evolutionary time. We observed and recorded the calling
patterns of the bird-voiced treefrog Hyla avivoca to assess how the dynamic characters of calls vary during interactions among groups of males in a chorus. This question was
also addressed using playback experiments with males. Playback experiments with females showed how changes in dynamic call
properties are likely to affect male mating success. Frogs calling in pairs, groups, or in response to playbacks produced
longer calls than did isolated males. During call overlap, males often increased the duration of the silent interval (gaps)
between the pulses of their calls so that the pulses of the calls of two neighbors interdigitated. This change resulted in
increased variability of pulse rate, a traditionally static acoustic property; however, males also produced high proportions
of non-overlapped calls in which variability in pulse rate was low and had species-typical values. Females preferred long
calls to short- and average-duration calls, and non-overlapped calls to overlapped calls. Given a choice between pairs of
overlapped calls, females preferred pairs in which the proportion of overlap was low and pairs in which the pulses of such
calls interdigitated completely. The observed patterns of vocal competition thus reflect the preferences of conspecific females,
which have influenced the evolution of the calling behavior of H. avivoca.
Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (doi:) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users. 相似文献
12.
Kelly M. Proffitt Robert A. Garrott Jay J. Rotella 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2008,62(10):1679-1687
The Trivers–Willard model predicts that in polygynous species, superior-quality females will maximize their fitness by producing
male offspring. Using a sample of 1,780 Weddell seal (Leptonychotes weddellii) pups recorded over 31 years, we investigated relationships between offspring sex ratio and maternal age, reproductive experience,
an index of maternal lifetime reproductive output, and annual environmental variations. We found evidence that females with
higher index of lifetime reproductive output were more likely to produce male than female offspring but found only weak evidence
that large-scale environmental variations influenced sex ratios. Our results suggest that mothers manipulate offspring sex
to maximize their own fitness, and inherent maternal quality may influence offspring sex. These findings support the Trivers–Willard
sex-allocation model.
Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (doi:) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users. 相似文献
13.
The Hamilton–Zuk hypothesis proposes that females prefer male secondary sexual traits because they are honest indicators of
parasite resistance. Despite the attention that this hypothesis has received, its role in sexual selection remains equivocal.
This study presents the first field test in guppies of two key predictions of the Hamilton–Zuk hypothesis: (1) that within
populations, the most highly ornamented males have the fewest parasites and (2) that among populations, males in high parasite
populations have the most conspicuous ornaments. Five hundred male guppies from 19 distinct populations in the Northern Range
of Trinidad were inspected for Gyrodactylus parasites and photographed. Eight measures of orange spot ornamentation were used to test the predictions: hue, saturation,
lightness, relative area, number, and area-weighted hue, saturation, and lightness. Parasite load had no significant effect
on any of these measures. There was also no relationship between orange spot ornamentation and parasite abundance among populations.
Guppies from high-predation environments had significantly more parasites, and their orange coloration was lighter and less
saturated than that in guppies from low-predation environments. Despite previous lab results, this study found no relationship
between parasite load and male orange spot ornamentation. 相似文献
14.
We investigated the effects of population fluctuation on the offspring’s sex allocation by a weakly polygynous mouse, Apodemus argenteus, for 3 years. In acorn-poor seasons, heavier mothers invested more in sons, and lighter mothers invested more in daughters.
In acorn-rich seasons, heavier mothers invested more in daughters, and lighter mothers invested more in sons. Maternal body
condition and litter size affected the sex allocation. Furthermore, there was a maternal investment trade-off between a son’s
birth mass and the number of daughters. Based upon the effect of population fluctuation on the lifetime reproductive success
of each sex, we proposed the new “safe bet hypothesis”. This hypothesis predicts that frequent and unpredictable change in
female distribution, which is often caused by abrupt fall in food condition, favors female-biased maternal investment to offspring
by polygynous mammals and is applicable to many small mammals inhabiting in unstable environments. 相似文献