全文获取类型
收费全文 | 40896篇 |
免费 | 353篇 |
国内免费 | 256篇 |
专业分类
安全科学 | 919篇 |
废物处理 | 1917篇 |
环保管理 | 5421篇 |
综合类 | 5989篇 |
基础理论 | 11801篇 |
环境理论 | 26篇 |
污染及防治 | 10124篇 |
评价与监测 | 2897篇 |
社会与环境 | 2199篇 |
灾害及防治 | 212篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 263篇 |
2021年 | 290篇 |
2020年 | 212篇 |
2019年 | 263篇 |
2018年 | 1827篇 |
2017年 | 1704篇 |
2016年 | 1765篇 |
2015年 | 688篇 |
2014年 | 814篇 |
2013年 | 2312篇 |
2012年 | 1385篇 |
2011年 | 2602篇 |
2010年 | 1765篇 |
2009年 | 1667篇 |
2008年 | 2138篇 |
2007年 | 2532篇 |
2006年 | 1275篇 |
2005年 | 1146篇 |
2004年 | 1173篇 |
2003年 | 1142篇 |
2002年 | 1117篇 |
2001年 | 1316篇 |
2000年 | 900篇 |
1999年 | 585篇 |
1998年 | 461篇 |
1997年 | 454篇 |
1996年 | 461篇 |
1995年 | 505篇 |
1994年 | 460篇 |
1993年 | 407篇 |
1992年 | 423篇 |
1991年 | 388篇 |
1990年 | 389篇 |
1989年 | 424篇 |
1988年 | 357篇 |
1987年 | 312篇 |
1986年 | 288篇 |
1985年 | 312篇 |
1984年 | 309篇 |
1983年 | 336篇 |
1982年 | 330篇 |
1981年 | 277篇 |
1980年 | 245篇 |
1979年 | 274篇 |
1978年 | 233篇 |
1977年 | 199篇 |
1976年 | 200篇 |
1975年 | 196篇 |
1974年 | 175篇 |
1972年 | 199篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
191.
192.
193.
194.
195.
196.
197.
DUNCAN S. WILSON‡ MARGO A. STODDARD† MATTHEW G. BETTS KLAUS J. PUETTMANN 《Conservation biology》2009,23(4):982-991
Abstract: Species conservation risk assessments require accurate, probabilistic, and biologically meaningful maps of population distribution. In patchy populations, the reasons for discontinuities are not often well understood. We tested a novel approach to habitat modeling in which methods of small area estimation were used within a hierarchical Bayesian framework. Amphibian occurrence was modeled with logistic regression that included third-order drainages as hierarchical effects to account for patchy populations. Models including the random drainage effects adequately represented species occurrences in patchy populations of 4 amphibian species in the Oregon Coast Range (U.S.A.). Amphibian surveys from other locations within the same drainage were used to calibrate local drainage-scale effects. Cross-validation showed that prediction errors for calibrated models were 77% to 86% lower than comparable regionally constructed models, depending on species. When calibration data were unavailable, small area and regional models performed similarly, although poorly. Small area estimation models complement wildlife ecology and habitat studies, and can help managers develop a regional picture of the conservation status for relatively rare species. 相似文献
198.
Heide Spiegel Manfred Sager Michael Oberforster Klemens Mechtler Hans Peter Stüger Andreas Baumgarten 《Environmental geochemistry and health》2009,31(5):549-560
Eighteen representative sites for the Austrian grain-growing and eight for the potato-growing zones (soils and crops) were investigated. On each site, total element contents (B, Ba, Ca, Cd, Co, Cu, Fe, K, Li, Mg, Mn, Mo, Na, P, Sr and Zn) were determined in 4–12 varieties of winter wheat (n = 136), 6 varieties of spring durum wheat (n = 30), 5 varieties of winter durum wheat (n = 15), 7 varieties of rye (n = 49), 5 varieties of spring barley (n = 30) and 5 varieties of potatoes (n = 40). Element accumulations in grain species and potato tubers varied significantly with site conditions, with the main exceptions for B in potatoes and wheat as well as for Zn, Cu and Co in durum wheat. On average, across all investigated sites, differences in varieties occurred concerning the elements Ca, Cd, Ba, Sr and Zn (except Zn in potatoes and winter durum). A rough estimation revealed that an average Austrian consumer of wheat, rye and potatoes meets more than 50% of the needs of daily element intake for K, P and Mg, between 36 and 72% for Fe, Zn and Cu, and more than 100% for Co, Mo and Mn. In particular, the elements Ca and Na have to be added from other sources. 相似文献
199.
Dror Hawlena 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2009,64(2):205-213
Colorful tails that become cryptic during ontogeny are found in diverse taxa. Nevertheless, the evolutionary bases for this change remain debated. Recent work suggests that colorful tails, deflective displays, and striped patterns may represent antipredator mechanisms used by immature lizards to compensate for being more active and hence more vulnerable to predation (increased movement hypothesis, IMH). I challenged the generality of IMH by comparing foraging behavior and frequency of tail displays across five Acanthodactylus lizards that vary in fundamental life history traits, before and after the tail changed color. As these species underwent changes in tail coloration, they congruently adopted less risky behaviors and reduced the frequencies of tail displays. Contrary to expectation, in two species, the hatchling risky behavior resulted not from increased movements but from longer stay in exposed microhabitats. I suggest that colorful tails and deflective tail displays are synergistic antipredator mechanisms neonates use to minimize the fitness consequences of using various risky behaviors rather than increased movement alone. 相似文献
200.
María del Mar Delgado Vincenzo Penteriani Vilis O. Nams Letizia Campioni 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2009,64(1):35-43
Moving and spatial learning are two intertwined processes: (a) changes in movement behavior determine the learning of the spatial environment, and (b) information plays a crucial role in several animal decision-making processes like movement decisions. A useful way to explore the interactions between movement decisions and learning of the spatial environment is by comparing individual behaviors during the different phases of natal dispersal (when individuals move across more or less unknown habitats) with movements and choices of breeders (who repeatedly move within fixed home ranges), that is, by comparing behaviors between individuals who are still acquiring information vs. individuals with a more complete knowledge of their surroundings. When analyzing movement patterns of eagle owls, Bubo bubo, belonging to three status classes (floaters wandering across unknown environments, floaters already settled in temporary settlement areas, and territory owners with a well-established home range), we found that: (1) wandering individuals move faster than when established in a more stable or fixed settlement area, traveling larger and straighter paths with longer move steps; and (2) when floaters settle in a permanent area, then they show movement behavior similar to territory owners. Thus, movement patterns show a transition from exploratory strategies, when animals have incomplete environmental information, to a more familiar way to exploit their activity areas as they get to know the environment better. 相似文献