首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   96篇
  免费   0篇
安全科学   2篇
环保管理   4篇
综合类   55篇
基础理论   20篇
污染及防治   14篇
社会与环境   1篇
  2017年   1篇
  2015年   1篇
  2013年   2篇
  2012年   3篇
  2010年   2篇
  2009年   2篇
  2008年   4篇
  2007年   4篇
  2006年   2篇
  2005年   2篇
  2004年   1篇
  2003年   4篇
  2002年   3篇
  2001年   1篇
  2000年   3篇
  1999年   2篇
  1998年   4篇
  1996年   2篇
  1995年   2篇
  1994年   1篇
  1993年   1篇
  1992年   1篇
  1991年   3篇
  1989年   1篇
  1988年   2篇
  1986年   2篇
  1983年   2篇
  1982年   1篇
  1981年   1篇
  1980年   1篇
  1967年   1篇
  1966年   4篇
  1965年   2篇
  1964年   2篇
  1963年   3篇
  1962年   2篇
  1961年   2篇
  1960年   2篇
  1959年   3篇
  1958年   4篇
  1957年   3篇
  1955年   1篇
  1954年   4篇
  1940年   1篇
  1937年   1篇
排序方式: 共有96条查询结果,搜索用时 46 毫秒
81.
Generalized Volterra models are analyzed qualitatively to provide some information concerning their applicability as ecological-model estimators. For the general two-population system (including prey-predator, competitive, and other cases), the stability of the equilibrium points and other properties of the phase representation are investigated. For n-population systems, stability analysis is carried out mainly by means of several Liapunov functions. The persistence (or global stability of nontrivial equilibrium) of n-population systems is investigated, leading to an interaction-exclusion principle which implies restrictions on the interaction structure of the system, especially in the case of trophic systems.  相似文献   
82.
Summary In many altricial species including the great tit (Parus major) the intensity of brood defense against predators has been shown to increase with the age of the offspring. This effect has been ascribed amongst others to the young becoming more vulnerable as they age (vulnerability hypothesis). In a great tit population suffering heavy losses from brood depredation by the great spotted woodpecker (Dendrocopus major), we rendered first and second broods more vulnerable by artificially enlarging the entrance of the nest hole. Contrary to the vulnerability hypothesis, 16 experimental pairs defended their brood against a dummy great spotted woodpecker less vigorously than did 16 control pairs. Nest concealment behavior potentially compromising active defense was minimized by simultaneous playback of nestling distress calls, thus simulating the act of nest predation. This leaves the brood value hypothesis as an alternative functional explanation of the defense level — age effect. It predicts that parents should defend their brood in proportion to the reproductive value (or some more suitable cohortal equivalent measure) of their offspring. At present, this explanation pertains to one predator species. In first broods, but not in second broods, males defended them more vigorously than did their females. While this parallels previous experiments on brood defense against predators posing a much greater risk to the parents, two functional explanations previously put forward can hardly apply.  相似文献   
83.
Double-choice experiments with three adult males of the little golden-mantled flying fox, Pteropus pumilus, and ten adult greater musky fruit bats, Ptenochirus jagori (both Megachiroptera: Pteropodidae), demonstrate that they are able to discriminate accurately between an empty dish and a dish containing fruits of one of several species by odour alone. Tests were run using fruits of six fruit species for Pteropus pumilus and five fruit species for Ptenochirus jagori. The fruit species used are known to be consumed in the wild by Ptenochirus jagori and are, with two exceptions, species of the natural rain-forest habitat. This is the first study to show that fruit bats are also able to assess the ripeness of a fruit exclusively by its odour. The bats preferred ripe over unripe fruits of the same species. Thus, both Pteropus pumilus and Ptenochirus jagori can not only locate fruits by their odour but can also discriminate between ripe and unripe fruits of the same species by olfaction. The results confirm and expand earlier findings on the role of olfactory cues in the orientation of foraging pteropodids.  相似文献   
84.
The scepticism prevailing with regard to the promotion of an elite in science--not, however, in the arts or in sports--originates from increasing mistrust vis-à-vis a new power based on knowledge held by certain social classes only. It has to be removed by opening the education system, by establishing new patterns of confidence between scientists and policy-makers, and by linking knowledge and responsibility. The confusion between elite and talent is another cause of this sceptical attitude. The task of education is not, however, to promote an elite, but to promote gifted pupils. Whether gifted young people will eventually become part of an élite is another question.  相似文献   
85.
86.
A fetus with mucopolysaccharidosis type IV A (Morquio type A) is described. The family had one affected child exhibiting symptoms of classical Morquio A disease, and late in the subsequent pregnancy prenatal diagnosis was requested. At 23 weeks' gestation, moderate ascites was detected by detailed ultrasound scan and keratan sulphate was found in the amniotic fluid. The pregnancy was terminated by prostaglandin induction and the diagnosis of mucopolysaccharidosis type IV A was confirmed by demonstration of a deficiency of N-acetylgalactosamine-6-sulphate (GalNac-6-S) sulphatase in cultured amniotic cells and in post-mortem fibroblast cultures. The activities of β-galactosidase and arylsulphatase A were normal, ruling out Morquio disease type B and multiple sulphatase deficiency. These results indicate that mucopolysaccharidosis IV A (a disease that predominantly affects the skeletal system) may produce ascites in the fetus to such an extent that it can be detected by ultrasound.  相似文献   
87.
88.
89.
90.
Summary Great tits (Parus major) tending nestlings reacted defensively to a live predator (Glaucidium perlatum; domestic cat) and the playback of a mixed species mobbing chorus, or to the latter alone. Defensive behaviour, mainly mobbing, reflected the risk taken and is assessed by five measures. Multivariate and contingency analyses revealed that at least 11 of 16 contextual independent variables affected the risk taken. Incremental effects are due to: Age of young, sex of the defending bird, the expected number of neighbouring mobbers, low temperature, wet canopy, the raptor's distance from cover, coniferous forest, advancing season. A decremental effect is exerted by a large brood that is older. Annual differences in defence arise probably from demographic factors such as fecundity, which in turn affect the parent's benefit-cost ratio (number of young of the same sex as the parent/residual reproductive value of the parent).While the effects of annual fecundity, age of young and season were predicted on the basis of this benefit-cost ratio, the failure to verify an incremental effect of brood size runs counter to established theory. We conclude that parents gear their defence efforts to energy investment, past or future, and are mal-adapted to brood size as a promotor of risk taken. The influence of the habitat is poorly understood. At least three factors (age and number of young, parent's sex) act additively on part of the response. Despite the large number of variables examined, about 43% of the total response variance remains unexplained.While four defence measures are determined by at least 10 contextual factors, a fifth measure, the male's minimum distance from the raptor, is determined by one other factor, the appearance of the male. The latter leads us to assume an additional, social rôle of brood defence.Risk-assessment by great tits leading to risk-aversive defence behaviour is governed by evolved restraints rather than by momentary constraints. Examples are provided by the effects of weather and cover.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号