首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   20235篇
  免费   284篇
  国内免费   354篇
安全科学   680篇
废物处理   877篇
环保管理   2882篇
综合类   3316篇
基础理论   5515篇
环境理论   7篇
污染及防治   5104篇
评价与监测   1178篇
社会与环境   1186篇
灾害及防治   128篇
  2022年   165篇
  2021年   180篇
  2020年   183篇
  2019年   146篇
  2018年   393篇
  2017年   375篇
  2016年   581篇
  2015年   374篇
  2014年   459篇
  2013年   1439篇
  2012年   1001篇
  2011年   1084篇
  2010年   693篇
  2009年   796篇
  2008年   875篇
  2007年   943篇
  2006年   792篇
  2005年   966篇
  2004年   1110篇
  2003年   942篇
  2002年   581篇
  2001年   675篇
  2000年   536篇
  1999年   344篇
  1998年   214篇
  1997年   238篇
  1996年   225篇
  1995年   274篇
  1994年   217篇
  1993年   210篇
  1992年   177篇
  1991年   190篇
  1990年   181篇
  1989年   186篇
  1988年   154篇
  1987年   133篇
  1986年   157篇
  1985年   143篇
  1984年   191篇
  1983年   148篇
  1982年   172篇
  1981年   158篇
  1980年   130篇
  1979年   149篇
  1978年   94篇
  1977年   98篇
  1975年   88篇
  1974年   95篇
  1973年   87篇
  1972年   98篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 46 毫秒
201.
Foraging desert ants, Cataglyphis fortis, encounter different sequences of visual landmarks while navigating by path integration. This paper explores the question whether the storage of landmark information depends on the context in which the landmarks are learned during an ant's foraging journey. Two experimental set-ups were designed in which the ants experienced an artificial landmark panorama that was placed either around the nest entrance (nest marks) or along the vector route leading straight towards the feeder (route marks). The two training paradigms resulted in pronounced differences in the storage characteristics of the acquired landmark information: memory traces of nest marks were much more robust against extinction and/or suppression than those of route marks. In functional terms, this result is in accord with the observation that desert ants encounter new route marks during every foraging run but always pass the same landmarks when approaching the nest entrance.  相似文献   
202.
Population differences in anti-predator behaviour have been demonstrated in several species, although less is known about the genetic basis of these traits. To determine the extent of genetic differences in boldness (defined as exploration of a novel object) and shoaling within and between zebrafish (Danio rerio) populations, and to examine the genetic basis of shoaling behaviour in general, we carried out a study that involved laboratory-raised fish derived from four wild-caught populations. Controlling for differences in rearing environment, significant inter-population differences were found in boldness but not shoaling. A larger shoaling experiment was also performed using one of the populations as the basis of a North Carolina type II breeding design (174 fish in total) to estimate heritability of shoaling tendency. A narrow-sense heritability estimate of 0.40 was obtained, with no apparent dominance effects.  相似文献   
203.
204.
205.
Egyptian Blue, a multicomponent synthetic blue pigment has been recorded in ancient Egypt since the Fourth Dynasty of the Old Kingdom (2600–2480 B.C.). The pigment consisting of cuprorivaite (CaCuSi4O10) with variable amounts of wollastonite (CaSiO3), Cu-rich glass and cuprite (Cu3O) or tenorite (CuO) was prepared by melting the copper-rich ingredient with lime and desert sand. Low melting temperatures (below 742 °C) were achieved by addition of flux-like plant ashes. The high quality of the pigments collected from monuments of the Fifth Dynasty (2480–2320 B.C.) may indicate that the first manufacture was in early dynastic or perhaps predynastic eras. During the reign of Thutmosis III (18th Dynasty, 1490–1436 B.C.) probably bronze filings were first applied as starting material, thus indicating a technological innovation. This new method was employed till the Roman times.  相似文献   
206.
The perception of pitch   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
  相似文献   
207.
208.
209.
210.
We present a technique to aspirate amniotic fluid from both sacs in biamniotic twin pregnancies using a single abdominal insertion with a spinal needle. It was successful in 48 out of 55 cases of biamniotic twin pregnancies referred to our perinatal unit between 1985 and 1994. The single insertion technique was used when the inter-amniotic membrane was clearly evident and two separate free amniotic fluid pools could be reached by the operator with a single puncture. An adequate amount of amniotic fluid was sampled from both sacs to make a cytogenetic diagnosis in all cases. There were four fetuses with trisomy 21 in three twin pregnancies. In two cases, only one twin was affected whilst the co-twin was normal, so that a selective feticide was performed. No miscarriages due to genetic amniocentesis were reported. After 1990, all genetic amniocenteses in biamniotic twin pregnancies (except for one case due to late booking) were performed between 14 and 15 weeks of gestation and with all cases except one, it was possible to sample both twins by a single puncture. We suggest that early amniocentesis (14–15 weeks) by a single abdominal puncture could be a reliable and safe alternative to first-trimester chorionic villus sampling in twin pregnancies.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号