排序方式: 共有147条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
101.
Craig A. Walling Nick J. Royle Neil B. Metcalfe Jan Lindström 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2007,61(7):1007-1014
It is well documented that poor early nutrition can have profound negative effects on adult life-history traits. However,
it has also been demonstrated that organisms can undergo compensatory resource allocation strategies (such as an accelerated
growth rate) if food availability improves, so as to mitigate the effects of the poor early conditions. Previous research
has indicated that elevated growth rates can incur costs in the longer term, such as an increased rate of senescence and shorter
lifespan. We tested whether a phase of compensatory growth after a period of reduced food availability earlier in life affected
the sexual attractiveness of adult male green swordtails Xiphophorus helleri, a species in which it has previously been documented that females prefer larger-bodied and longer-tailed males. The experiment
compared the attractiveness of size-matched brothers that had experienced contrasting growth trajectories as juveniles; the
experiments were initially conducted in the middle of a male’s sexually reproductive life and were then repeated towards the
very end of life. At both ages, males that had undergone compensatory growth were equally as attractive as their brothers
that had grown normally. These results suggest that the growth compensation benefits males through an increase in their attractiveness
over that which they would have had if they had remained on their original growth trajectory. The lack of change in relative
attractiveness with age indicates that the compensation does not cause greater deterioration in secondary sexual characters
at older ages than in continuously well-fed males. 相似文献
102.
Contrary to classical sexual selection theories, females of many taxa mate with multiple males during one reproductive cycle. In this study, we conducted an experiment on the “trade-up hypothesis”, which proposes that females remate if a subsequently encountered male is potentially superior to previous mates to maximize the genetic quality of their offspring. We presented bank vole females (Clethrionomys glareolus) sequentially with two males of known dominance rank in different orders, i.e., either first subordinate and second dominant, first dominant and second subordinate, or two males that were equal in dominance (high ranking) and observed their mating behavior. We found that 92% of the females mated multiply and did not base their remating decision on male social status. Therefore, polyandry cannot be explained by the “trade-up hypothesis” based on dominance rank in this species. However, we found that dominant males sired significantly more offspring than subordinate males. This varied according to mating order: dominant males sired more offspring when they were second than when they were first. Moreover, litter sizes were significantly smaller when the dominant male was first (smallest relative success of dominant males) compared to litter sizes when mating order was reversed or both males equal in status. Our results suggest that even though multimale mating includes males that are of poorer quality and thus potentially decreases the fitness of offspring, most of a female’s offspring are sired by dominant males. Whether this is due to cryptic female choice, sperm competition, or a combination of both, remains to be tested. 相似文献
103.
Costs of searching for a mate are an important component of models of sexual selection, yet they have rarely been examined in wild populations of vertebrates. In this paper, we report an experiment in which we handicapped female tree swallows by clipping some flight feathers. This manipulation increased the costs of flight and searching for extra-pair mates. Despite these costs, handicapped females had the same level of extra-pair mating (percentage of extra-pair young, percentage of broods with extra-pair young, and the number of extra-pair sires per brood) as control females. However, handicapped females were more likely to have young sired by extra-pair males that lived closer to her nest than control females. This change in the distribution of extra-pair mating was most likely due to female choice rather than male coercion, and it suggests that extra-pair mating has significant benefits to females. One important implication of our study is that ecological and social factors that influence search costs could affect the spatial distribution of extra-pair sires and, consequently, the intensity of sexual selection. These effects may have been overlooked in previous studies that did not identify extra-pair sires. 相似文献
104.
在广泛,全面收集有关环境保护的国家和地方法律,法规和政策标准等信息资料的基础上,运用微机的资料信息数据编程技术,开发环保法律法规标准政策法规微机查询系统,具有目录式查询,索引式查询,正文关键词自由查询和法规条文解释查阅等功能。 相似文献
105.
Female mallard mating preferences for multiple male ornaments 总被引:3,自引:1,他引:3
K. E. Omland 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1996,39(6):353-360
Mating preferences were studied in captive wild-stock female mallards. Independent observers scored ornament brightness for
the bill and eight plumage areas of male mallards on a scale of 1 to 6. Ornament scores were generally positively correlated
with each other as indicated by univariate correlations and principal component analysis. Males with higher bill scores were
significantly more likely to obtain pairings. None of eight individual plumage ornaments were significant predictors of pairing
success, but males with higher average plumage scores had significantly higher pairing success. Males that completed the molt
first were also significantly more likely to be paired. Thus females may have paid some attention to plumage brightness. Stepwise
multiple regression produced a model that included only bill and molt, both of which contributed a similar degree (r
2 = 0.39); plumage ornaments and size measurements did not enter the model. These results are interpreted in light of hypotheses
for the evolution of multiple ornaments, and phylogenetic plumage patterns in dabbling ducks.
Received: 18 December 1995 / Accepted after revision: 22 September 1996 相似文献
106.
Ornamentation predicts reproductive success in female pipefish 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
Anders Berglund Gunilla Rosenqvist Patricia Bernet 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1997,40(3):145-150
In the sex-role reversed pipefish Syngnathus typhle females compete for access to males and males are choosy. Females develop a temporary ornament when competing over mates
with other females and when performing nuptial dances with males. This ornament is an amplification of the normal striped
pattern in these fishes. We here show experimentally that (1) the contrast of this normal pattern forecasts the extent to
which the ornament is shown, (2) contrast and ornamentation honestly signal female quality (egg numbers), (3) contrast and
ornamentation accurately predict female mating success, (4) contrast is a phenotypically plastic trait specifically exaggerated
under situations of female – female competition, and (5) neither contrast nor ornament are energetically expensive to the
females (i.e., they are independent of short-term nutritional status). Hence, as predicted in sex-role reversed species, ornament
design is constrained by costs to female fecundity: an energetically demanding ornament would impair on a female's ability
to produce eggs. The type of ornament described here is the expected one, costly for reasons other than being energetically
expensive to produce.
Received: 4 April 1996 / Accepted after revision: 27 October 1996 相似文献
107.
Tom Tregenza Fathi Attia Saleh Salem Bushaiba 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2009,63(6):817-823
Differences among males in their success in achieving fertilisations when females mate with more than one partner are now
recognised as an important target of sexual selection. However, very few studies have attempted to determine whether particular
males are consistently successful in sperm competition and whether success in sperm competition is a heritable trait. Additionally,
the potential heritability of female traits that influence the outcome of sperm competition has received only limited attention.
Using the polyandrous beetle Tribolium castaneum, we examined repeatability of male success in sperm competition by mating pairs of males carrying different visible genetic
markers to a string of different females. Males showed consistency in their ability to successfully transfer sperm to females,
but not in their success in sperm competition. Furthermore, when we independently compared success in sperm competition of
fathers with their sons, we found no evidence for heritability of this trait. Similarly, females that exhibited high or low
first male sperm precedence did not tend to have daughters that showed the same pattern. Our results suggest that we should
be wary of assuming that success in sperm competition is heritable through either sex. 相似文献
108.
Kathrin P. Lampert Ximena E. Bernal A. Stanley Rand Ulrich G. Mueller Michael J. Ryan 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2006,59(6):796-804
In most sexually reproducing animals, the behavior of one or both sexes during courtship critically influences the success
at mating of the opposite sex. This behavior is often interpreted as “mate choice,” and there is great interest in why such
choices are exercised. The explanation for the evolution of mate choice that has received the most attention and generated
the most controversy is based on assumed genetic effects. In this study, we investigated whether female túngara frogs, which
choose mates based on acoustic cues, have a preference for genetically less related males. Specifically, we determine if there
is disassortive mating based on microsatellite markers, if there is information in the advertisement call that could be used
to assess genetic similarity, and if females exhibit acoustic-based mating preferences that would promote choice for genetic
diversity. Using seven microsatellite markers, we found no correlation of male call similarity and male genetic relatedness.
Female choice experiments showed no female preference for calls of less related males, and there was no evidence for inbreeding
avoidance in the field. Our results do not support the hypothesis of mate choice based on information about genetic relatedness
conveyed by acoustic signals in túngara frogs. 相似文献
109.
Sperm exchange in a simultaneous hermaphrodite 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3
Many simultaneously hermaphroditic animals cross-inseminate when copulating, even though unilateral sperm exchange is functionally
possible. The question is why reciprocity is the rule. This study addresses sperm exchange in a hermaphroditic planarian flatworm,
Dugesia polychroa, in which self-fertilisation does not occur. We investigated the availability of self sperm for inseminating a partner, how
self sperm affects the likelihood of sperm donation, and whether sperm donation depends on reciprocation by the partner. The
amount of self sperm depended on body size, duration of isolation and experimental date, and animals that had more self sperm
were more likely to mate. Depletion of allosperm (sperm received from partners) played only an indirect role, suggesting that
animals copulate more to donate sperm rather than to replenish allosperm reserves. Among 60 copulating pairs, reciprocal insemination
was more common (63.3%) than unilateral transfer (18.3%). A surprising 18.3% did not exchange sperm in either direction. Individuals
were more likely to behave like their partners, resulting in more symmetrical matings (either reciprocity or no sperm exchange)
than asymmetrical matings (unilateral sperm donation). The amount of self sperm donated during a copulation depended mainly
on the amount available before the copulation and not on the amount received from the partner. The results suggest that conditional
sperm exchange, or sperm trading, takes place in D. polychroa.
Received: 31 May 1997 / Accepted after revision: 1 December 1997 相似文献
110.
Y. Tsubaki Michael T. Siva-Jothy Tomohiro Ono 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1994,35(4):219-225
After copulation, male Nannophya pygmaea dragonflies mate guard by hovering over ovipositing females and repelling conspecific males. Copulation is not always a prerequisite for oviposition in the females of this species because females can store the sperm received during previous visits/copulations. An oviposition episode consists of several bouts of oviposition separated by periods of perching. We conducted two types of male-removal experiments to examine the effects of mating and post-copulatory mate guarding on the oviposition behaviour of females. In the first experiment, we removed all males from the habitat to eliminate the effect of re-copulation, mate-guarding and harassment by males. In the second experiment, we removed males immediately after copulation to eliminate the effects of guarding and other post-copulatory male-female interactions. We compared these experimental data with data obtained under natural conditions. The dipping rate in an oviposition bout was not influenced by copulation or guarding. However, guarded females made more dips per episode than did solitary females. The proportion of time actually spent ovipositing (total bout duration/oviposition episode duration) of guarded females was higher than that of solitary females. Solitary females often oviposited in more than one territorial site, while guarded females usually oviposited within a single territorial site during an oviposition episode. Because males tend to hold territories at sites where egg survival is high, guarded females (and the male guardian) benefit from guarding in terms of egg hatchability. The possible benefits for solitary females are discussed. 相似文献