全文获取类型
收费全文 | 120篇 |
免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
安全科学 | 10篇 |
废物处理 | 1篇 |
环保管理 | 16篇 |
综合类 | 18篇 |
基础理论 | 36篇 |
污染及防治 | 2篇 |
评价与监测 | 5篇 |
社会与环境 | 30篇 |
灾害及防治 | 3篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 1篇 |
2020年 | 2篇 |
2019年 | 4篇 |
2018年 | 2篇 |
2017年 | 2篇 |
2016年 | 3篇 |
2014年 | 1篇 |
2013年 | 31篇 |
2012年 | 2篇 |
2011年 | 4篇 |
2010年 | 1篇 |
2009年 | 6篇 |
2008年 | 4篇 |
2007年 | 7篇 |
2006年 | 10篇 |
2005年 | 4篇 |
2004年 | 6篇 |
2003年 | 3篇 |
2002年 | 2篇 |
2001年 | 4篇 |
2000年 | 4篇 |
1999年 | 4篇 |
1998年 | 5篇 |
1997年 | 1篇 |
1996年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 1篇 |
1994年 | 2篇 |
1993年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有121条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
41.
42.
Honey bee foragers as sensory units of their colonies 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
Thomas D. Seeley 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》1994,34(1):51-62
Forager honey bees function not only as gatherers of food for their colonies, but also as sensory units shaped by natural selection to gather information regarding the location and profitability of forage sites. They transmit this information to colony members by means of waggle dances. To investigate the way bees transduce the stimulus of nectar-source profitability into the response of number of waggle runs, I performed experiments in which bees were stimulated with a sucrose solution feeder of known profitability and their dance responses were videorecorded. The results suggest that several attributes of this transduction process are adaptations to enhance a bee's effectiveness in reporting on a forage site. (1) Bees register the profitability of a nectar source not by sensing the energy gain per foraging trip or the rate of energy gain per trip, but evidently by sensing the energetic efficiency of their foraging. Perhaps this criterion of nectar-source profitability has been favored by natural selection because the foraging gains of honey bees are typically limited by energy expenditure rather than time availability. (2) There is a linear relationship between the stimulus of energetic efficiency of foraging and the response of number of waggle runs per dance. Such a simple stimulus-response function appears adequate because the range of suprathreshold stimuli (max/min ratio of about 10) is far smaller than the range of responses (max/min ratio of about 100). Although all bees show a linear stimulus-response function, there are large differences among individuals in both the response threshold and the slope of the stimulus-response function. This variation gives the colony a broader dynamic range in responding to food sources than if all bees had identical thresholds of dance response. (3) There is little or no adaptation in the dance response to a strong stimulus (tonic response). Thus each dancing bee reports on the current level of profitability of her forage site rather than the changes in its profitability. This seems appropriate since presumably it is the current profitability of a forage site, not the change in its profitability, which determines a site's attractiveness to other bees. (4) The level of forage-site quality that is the threshold for dancing is tuned by the bees in relation to forage availability. Bees operate with a lower dance threshold when forage is sparse than when it is abundant. Thus a colony utilizes input about a wide range of forage sites when food is scarce, but filters out input about low-reward sites when food is plentiful. (5) A dancing bee does not present her information in one spot within the hive but instead distributes it over much of the dance floor. Consequently, the dances for different forage sites are mixed together on the dance floor. This helps each bee following the dances to take a random sample of the dance information, which is appropriate for the foraging strategy of a honey bee colony since it is evidently designed to allocate foragers among forage sites in proportion to their profitability. 相似文献
43.
The term governance describes the multitude of actors and processes that lead to collectively binding decisions. The term
risk governance translates the core principles of governance to the context of risk-related policy making. We aim to delineate
some basic lessons from the insights of the other articles in this special issue for our understanding of risk governance.
Risk governance provides a conceptual as well as normative basis for how to cope with uncertain, complex and/or ambiguous
risks. We propose to synthesize the breadth of the articles in this special issue by suggesting some changes to the risk governance
framework proposed by the International Risk Governance Council (IRGC) and adding some insights to its analytical and normative
implications. 相似文献
44.
Introduction: Injuries and work-related musculoskeletal disorders (MSDs) are common among masons. SAfety Voice for Ergonomics (SAVE) integrates training in ergonomic and safety problem-solving skills into masonry apprenticeship training. The purpose of this study was to assess the efficacy of text messaging to reinforce SAVE program content. Method: SAVE effectiveness was evaluated at masonry apprenticeship training centers across the United States by comparing three experimental groups: (1) Ergonomics training, (2) Ergonomics and Safety Voice training, and a (3) Control. Apprentices received SAVE training with their standard instruction. To reinforce classroom training, refresher training was implemented by sending weekly text messages for six months. Half of the text messages required a response, which tested knowledge or assessed behavior, while the remaining reiterated knowledge. Apprentices (n = 119) received SAVE text messages. Response rates and percentage of correct responses were compared with chi-square tests and independent group t-tests. Multivariable logistic regression analysis predicted apprentice response with selected demographic and work experience variables. Finally, feedback on of the use of text messaging was obtained. Result: Of 119 participants, 61% (n = 72) responded to at least one text message. Logistic regression revealed that being a high school graduate and a brick and block mason significantly affected the odds of responding. Sixty-nine percent of apprentices agreed that text messages reinforced SAVE content. Conclusion: Even though there was no training center requirement to respond, the high response rate suggests that text messaging can effectively be used to reinforce ergonomics and safety voice training for both knowledge and behavior. Practical Application: The prevalent use of text messaging creates opportunities to reinforce health and safety training and engage workers, especially for populations that may be at various locations over time such as construction sites. Instructors and practitioners should consider the utility of text messaging for supporting their training and safety programs. 相似文献
45.
Dale E. Heydlauff 《Environmental management》1996,20(6):963-966
The quality and sustainability of the natural environment is a matter of inestimable value and is critical to public health and welfare. All species have a purpose, and they exist for the betterment of other species. It is, therefore, incumbent on all humans to do their part in the preservation of this vast, diverse ecosystem called Earth. All humans are the beneficiaries, the ultimate customers, of a sound environment—water that is safe to drink, air that can be breathed, and soil that will sustain crops. There must be a commitment to leaving a clean and healthy planet for generations to follow, an earth which is enhanced, not diminished, by humans' presence. 相似文献
46.
47.
48.
Lynn E. Fletcher 《Behavioral ecology and sociobiology》2007,61(12):1809-1821
Group living confers both benefits and costs to the individuals involved. Benefits may include enhanced defense, thermoregulation,
and increased foraging efficiency while costs often involve competition for resources such as food, shelter, and mates. Communication
provides a medium of exchange among individuals engaged in either cooperative or competitive interactions. The functional
analysis of signals within groups therefore requires testing both cooperative and competitive functions, although the latter
is infrequently done. In this paper, I study the use of two vibrational signals in a gregarious, processionary Australian
sawfly larva, Perga affinis: tapping and contractions. Tapping involves striking the substrate with the sclerotized portion of the abdominal tail and
a contraction is a fast, whole-body twitch, which is both tactile and vibrational in its transmission. For tapping, I first
demonstrate that it is a form of communication, as tapping of one larva elicits tapping in another, and that it is transmitted
through substrate vibrations. I then test whether the signal is mostly cooperative or competitive in nature by examining it
in light of two hypotheses: (1) the Group Coordination hypothesis, stating that the signal functions to maintain group cohesiveness
and (2) the Competitive Signaling hypothesis, stating that tapping serves as a competitive assessment signal between larvae
while feeding. For contractions, I test only the group coordination hypothesis that they serve to coordinate and initiate
group movement. Results support the group coordination hypothesis for each signal. While feeding, lone larvae (without potential
competitors) were significantly more likely to tap than those in groups, and this trend continued in non-feeding situations.
Contractions regularly preceded periods of group movement during processions and were given with increasing frequency before
departure from preforaging clusters. The vibrational signals in this processionary species likely function cooperatively to
maintain group cohesiveness and coordinate movement. 相似文献
49.
In order to remain stable, dispersed social groups have to solve two fundamental problems: the coordination of movement and cohesiveness within a group and the spacing between the groups. Here, we investigate mechanisms involved in intra-group coordination and inter-group spacing using the golden brown mouse lemur, Microcebus ravelobensis, as a model for a nocturnal, solitary foraging mammal with a dispersed social system. By means of radiotelemetry and bioacoustics we studied the olfactory and vocal behaviour during nocturnal dispersal and reunion of five sleeping groups.All groups used 3–17 sleeping sites exclusively, suggesting a sleeping site-related territoriality and competition for them. The occurrence of olfactory and vocal behaviour showed an asymmetrical temporal distribution. Whereas marking behaviour was observed exclusively during dispersal, a particular call type, the trill, was used by all groups during reunions. Interestingly, these trills carried group-specific signatures.Our findings provide the first empirical evidence for nocturnal primates in a natural environment that olfactory signals represent an important mechanism to regulate the distribution of different groups in space, whereas acoustic signals control intra-group cohesion and coordination. 相似文献
50.
Social insect colonies need to explore and exploit multiple food sources simultaneously and efficiently. At the individual
level, this colony-level behaviour has been thought to be taken care of by two types of individual: scouts that independently
search for food, and recruits that are directed by nest mates to a food source. However, recent analyses show that this strict
division of labour between scouts and recruits is untenable. Therefore, a modified concept is presented here that comprises
the possible behavioural states of an individual forager (novice forager, scout, recruit, employed forager, unemployed experienced
forager, inspector and reactivated forager) and the transitions between them. The available empirical data are reviewed in
the light of both the old and the new concept, and probabilities for the different transitions are derived for the case of
the honey-bee. The modified concept distinguishes three types of foragers that may be involved in the exploration behaviour
of the colony: novice bees that become scouts, unemployed experienced bees that scout, and lost recruits, i.e. bees that discover
a food source other than the one to which they were directed to by their nest mates. An advantage of the modified concept
is that it allows for a better comparison of studies investigating the different roles performed by social insect foragers
during their individual foraging histories.
Received: 29 December 1999 / Revised: 25 February 2000 / Accepted: 16 October 2000 相似文献